Sie sind auf Seite 1von 21

Child Development, March/April 2007, Volume 78, Number 2, Pages 681 – 701

Are There Long-Term Effects of Early Child Care?


Jay Belsky Deborah Lowe Vandell
Birkbeck University of London University of California, Irvine

Margaret Burchinal K. Alison Clarke-Stewart


University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill University of California, Irvine

Kathleen McCartney Margaret Tresch Owen


Harvard University The University of Texas, Dallas

The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network

Effects of early child care on children’s functioning from 412 years through the end of 6th grade (M age 5 12.0
years) were examined in the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child
Care and Youth Development (n 5 1,364). The results indicated that although parenting was a stronger and more
consistent predictor of children’s development than early child-care experience, higher quality care predicted
higher vocabulary scores and more exposure to center care predicted more teacher-reported externalizing
problems. Discussion focuses on mechanisms responsible for these effects, the potential collective consequences
of small child-care effects, and the importance of the ongoing follow-up at age 15.

Large numbers of children in the United States ex- children’s functioning are of great interest to parents,
perience routine nonmaternal child care during their educators, and policymakers, especially as heated
infant, toddler, and preschool years. In 1999, 9.8 debate has often characterized discussion of child-
million American children under the age of five care effectsFboth before the onset of the work pre-
years were in child care for 40 or more hours a week sented in this article (e.g., Belsky, 1986, 1988; Clarke-
(Committee on Family and Work Policies, 2003), with Stewart, 1989; Fox & Fein, 1990; Phillips, McCartney,
many beginning in the first year of life (U.S. Bureau Scarr, & Howes, 1987) and more recently (e.g., Bel-
of the Census, 1999). Questions about possible long- sky, 2001; Crockenberg, 2003; Greenspan, 2003;
term effects of early child care on school-aged Langlois & Liben, 2003; Maccoby & Lewis, 2003;
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network
[ECCRN], 2003a). To be clear, in all discussion that
follows, the language of child-care ‘‘effects’’ refers to
This study is directed by a steering committee and supported by statistical associations discerned in field studies be-
NICHD through a cooperative agreement (U10) that calls for a tween measurements of child-care experience and
scientific collaboration between the grantees and NICHD staff. child development, typically after controlling for
Participating investigators on the NICHD Early Child Care Re-
search Network, listed in alphabetical order, are: Jay Belsky,
confounding variables. Widely appreciated by the
Birkbeck University of London; Cathryn Booth-LaForce, Univer- authors of this report and investigators in the field is
sity of Washington; Robert Bradley, University of Arkansas, Little that such correlational research does not allow
Rock; Margaret Burchinal, University of North Carolina, Chapel strong inferences regarding causation as efforts to
Hill; Susan B. Campbell, University of Pittsburgh; K. Alison control confounding factors can never insure that all
Clarke-Stewart, University of California, Irvine; Sarah L. Fried-
important ‘‘third variables’’ or alternative explan-
man, Institute for Public Research, CAN Corporation, Alexandria,
Virginia, Maryland; Kathryn Hirsh-Pasek, Temple University; ations have been taken into account. Although ef-
Aletha Huston, University of Texas, Austin; Kathleen McCartney, forts are made in this paper to avoid causal
Harvard University; Marion O’Brien, University of North Caro- language, whenever the term ‘‘effects’’ is used to
lina, Greensboro; Margaret Tresch Owen, University of Texas, describe results it refers to statistical effects of child-
Dallas; Robert Pianta, University of Virginia; and Susan Spieker,
care predictor variables in this and related nonex-
University of Washington; Deborah Lowe Vandell, University of
California, Irvine. perimental field studies.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Jay Belsky, Institute for the Study of Children, Families and Social
Issues, Birkbeck University of London, 7 Bedford Square, London r 2007 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
WC1B 3RA, UK. Electronic mail may be sent to j.belsky@bbk.ac.uk. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2007/7802-0020
682 Belsky et al.

Two issues have been central to the debate about experience and child development. Especially be-
child-care ‘‘effects,’’ one pertaining to the specificity of cause definition would affect which child care ar-
effects and the other to the endurance of effects. Some rangements wereFand were notFstudied, the
have highlighted potentially beneficial consequences decision was made to define child care broadly, such
of early child care, especially of high-quality care, that any and all nonmaternal care that was regularly
on social functioning (e.g., Howes, 1988; Peisner- scheduled for at least 10 hr per week qualified as
Feinberg & Burchinal, 1997; Vandell, Henderson, & ‘‘child care,’’ including care by fathers, grandparents,
Wilson, 1988), as well as on cognitive-linguistic de- and other relatives. Ultimately, the decision was
velopment or academic achievement (e.g., Broberg, made to be inclusive because of a desire not to
Wessels, Lamb, & Hwang, 1997; Burchinal et al., privilege the biological relatedness of the caregiver
2000; Peisner-Feinberg et al., 2001), particularly in or the location of the care in the sense that certain
the case of economically disadvantaged children at- arrangements would not qualify as child care.
tending high-quality early-intervention programs Whereas some might contend, not unreasonably, that
(e.g., Campbell et al., 2001; Reynolds, 2000; Sch- nonmaternal care provided by the father or by the
weinhart, Weikart, & Larner, 1986). Others have grandmother should not be regarded as child care, it
called attention to potentially adverse consequences, seemed questionable that care provided by a
especially of long hours of care initiated early in life, grandmother in her own home was somehow less
on socioemotional functioning, including behavior ‘‘child care’’ than was care provided by a nanny
problems (e.g., Bates et al., 1994; Belsky, 1990, 2001; living full time in the child’s home or by a babysitter
Haskins, 1985; Vandell & Corasaniti, 1990). More- who came to the child’s home to provide care, es-
over, some have contended that child-care ‘‘effects,’’ pecially from the perspective of the infant receiving
whether beneficial or detrimental, do not endure the care.
beyond the preschool or early elementary school In prior published work, the NICHD ECCRN
years (e.g., Blau, 1999; Colwell, Pettit, Meece, Bates, (2002, 2003a, 2003b, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c, 2005a) re-
& Dodge, 2001; Deater-Deckard, Pinkerton, & Scarr, ported that all three of the aforementioned core
1996; Egeland & Hiester, 1995), whereas others have features of early child careFquality, quantity, and
claimed that child-care ‘‘effects’’ are more long last- typeFwere related to children’s school readiness
ing (e.g., Belsky, 1988; Vandell et al., 1988; Vandell & and social behavior measured at age 412 years, just
Corasaniti, 1990) or endure for some time, at least in before the transition to school. Higher quality child
the case of low-income children in high-quality care care predicted higher levels of preacademic skills
(Campbell et al., 2001; Lazar & Darlington, 1982; and language performance, although not social
Schweinhart et al., 1986). functioning, whereas more hours in care and in-
The National Institute of Child Health and Hu- creasing hours in care predicted higher levels of
man Development (NICHD) Study of Early Child behavior problems, but not academic skills or lan-
Care and Youth Development (SECCYD), launched guage functioning. Intriguingly, greater exposure to
in the early 1990s, was designed to address these center-type care proved to be related to child func-
issues while overcoming many of the design limita- tioning in both positive and negative ways, pre-
tions of prior work (NICHD ECCRN, 2005a). One of dicting better language skills and performance on a
the foremost limitations was the inability of most memory task, but also more problem behaviors (see
previous research to distinguish and thereby disen- also NICHD ECCRN, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c, 2006).
tangle potentially distinctive (statistical) effects of These associations between experience in child care
different features of the child-care experience, par- and child development were reliable, albeit small by
ticularly the quality of the care, the amount or quan- most standards. Nevertheless, there is great interest
tity of care, and the type of care. Virtually all research in the potential long-term sequelae of child-care ex-
before the NICHD SECCYDFand some more recent perience for many reasons, including the fact that
work as well (e.g., Bacharach & Baumeister, 2003; extensive early child care is a relatively new experi-
Borge, Rutter, Cote, & Tremblay, 2004)Fexamined ence for children in our culture, coupled with the fact
one or another feature of the child-care experience, that child-care experience is now normative for U.S.
but never all three. children.
At the initiation of the NICHD SECCYD, the When children in the NICHD SECCYD were fol-
collaborating investigators engaged in extensive lowed-up through third grade ( 8 years), some of
discussion about how to define ‘‘child care’’ oper- the ‘‘effects’’ detected at 54 months endured, some
ationally in order to address relations between vari- disappeared, and others emerged for the first time
ation in these aspects of the early child-care (NICHD ECCRN, 2005b). More specifically, higher
Early Child Care 683

quality care continued to be linked to higher scores fourth grade). With these longitudinal assessments,
on standardized tests of math, memory, and vo- we are positioned to determine whether findings
cabulary skills. More time spent in child care no pertaining to the quality, quantity, and type of child
longer predicted externalizing behavior problems, care detected at age 412 years are maintained, in-
but in contrast to findings at 54 months, was asso- crease, or decrease across the first 7 years of school
ciated with lower social competence and poorer (i.e., K – 6) in a relatively large and diverse sample.
academic work habits, the latter being an outcome of We also consider the possibility that previously un-
child care examined for the first time when children detected associations involving child care and child
were in third grade. Finally, more time periods of development may emerge. Not only is it the case that
center care continued to relate to children’s devel- some linkages between child care and child devel-
opment in both positive and negative ways, being opment (i.e., quantity:problem behavior) that were
associated with better memory, but also with more evident in the NICHD SECC at one age (i.e., 24
conflicted relationships with teachers and mothers. months) and disappeared at a later age (i.e., 36
As before, then, the third-grade follow-up high- months) reappeared thereafter (i.e., 54 months), but
lighted the relative independence of quality, quan- others, too, have detected significant child-care
tity, and type of child care in relation to children’s findings at a later age that were not present earlier
development. (i.e., Broberg et al., 1997).
The purpose of the current study is to extend our In all nonexperimental studies of child care, se-
ongoing research linking early child-care experience lection bias is an issue because family and child
with child functioning through sixth grade, in order characteristics are related to child outcomes as well
to determine whether the earlier detected associ- as the type, amount, and quality of care children
ations involving the quality, quantity, and type of experience (Committee on Family and Work Policies,
care change over time. To our knowledge, the only 2003). To reduce this problem, relations between
other investigations that have involved such long- child-care experience and child development are
term evaluations of samples first studied in infancy tested in the current study after controlling for an
were carried out in Sweden, where the ecology of extensive array of family factors. A related concern is
child care is markedly different from the United that associations between child care and child func-
States (e.g., better paid staff, less staff turnover, tioning may be explained by subsequent experien-
higher quality care; Andersson, 1992; Broberg et al., ces. In the case of school-aged children, concurrent
1997) or, in the United States, involved children en- experiences at home and in school, or the amount of
rolled in a particular high-quality, center-based, time spent in an afterschool program, may account
early-intervention programs intended to compensate for developmental trajectories during the primary
for disadvantages in the home (e.g., Campbell et al., grades, rather than children’s earlier child-care his-
2001; Lazar & Darlington, 1982; Reynolds, 2000). As a tories. For example, quality of classroom instruction
result, the work reported here breaks new ground by in the primary grades may eliminate earlier associ-
tracking American children to ages 11 – 12 and ations linking child-care quality with children’s
examining how variation in the type, quality, and cognitive-linguistic development. Additionally, large
quantity of care of the kind typically experienced in amounts of after-school care during the primary
communities across this country is associated with grades may account for behavior problems in the
cognitive development, achievement, and socio- primary grades rather than earlier child-care hours
emotional functioning. or experience of center-based care. Consequently, in
The NICHD SECCYD is well suited to address the current study, measures of the quality of early
issues of the long-term correlates of child-care ex- and concurrent parenting, of the quality of classroom
perience because information about child-care use instruction in the primary grades, and of the amount
was collected every 3 – 4 months from the time in- of out-of-school care in the primary grades were also
fants were 1 month of age until school entry. Quality included as covariates.
of the children’s primary child-care setting (e.g., Recently, van IJzendoorn et al. (2004) carried out a
center, family day-care home, nanny in own home) re-analysis of data from the NICHD SECCYD linking
was assessed using specially developed observa- a large amount of time spent in care with higher
tional methods when children were 6, 15, 24, 36, and externalizing problem scores at 54 months of age
54 months of age. Measures of cognitive and social (NICHD ECCRN, 2003a, 2003b, 2003c). They found
functioning were collected longitudinally at 412 years that the putative effect of long hours in any type
and in first, third, fifth, and sixth grade (and, for of care was a function of time spent in the care
some measures, also in kindergarten, second, and of nonrelatives (i.e., nannies, babysitters, day-care
684 Belsky et al.

homes, centers) and not time spent in care provided data tended to be from families with more income, to
by relatives (i.e., fathers, grandparents), and that it have parents who provided more responsive and
was center-based care in particular that most stimulating care, to experience more center care and
strongly related to problem behavior. Therefore, in spend more time in child care, and to show higher
the present work, we include a series of secondary academic achievement scores over time than did
analyses to ascertain whether findings pertaining to children with missing data. Children with and
amount of time in any kind of care vary as a function without missing data did not differ reliably on child-
of whether care is provided by a relative or non- care quality or teacher ratings.
relative, as the primary analyses focus upon the
distinction between center- and home-based care
Measures
and on overall quantity of care, just as it has in prior
reports by our research team (NICHD ECCRN, Measurements are described in terms of their
2003a, 2003b, 2003c, 2005a, 2005b, 2006). roles in the analyses to be reported. Measures re-
A second set of follow-up analyses explores a flecting the child’s experiences in child care before
delimited set of interactions. In particular, we ask school entry are described first. The variables used to
whether links between amount and type of care and control for family factors (i.e., family covariates) are
externalizing problems are more pronounced for described next. Then measures used to control for
boys than for girls and whether the relations linking school and after-school experiences are described.
quality of care with academic achievement are more Finally, we describe child social and cognitive out-
pronounced among children growing up in low-in- come measures. Information about this public data
come households. Although the NICHD SECCYD set can be found at http://secc.rti.org/.
has not found support for these moderated effects
before (NICHD ECCRN, 2000, 2002, 2003a, 2003b),
Child-Care Characteristics
they merit consideration given enduring hypotheses
about their importance (Crockenberg, 2003; Love Nonmaternal child care was defined, as already
et al., 2003; Maccoby & Lewis, 2003). noted, as regular care by anyone other than the
motherFincluding fathers, relatives, and nannies
(whether in home or out of home), family day-care
Method
providers, and centers. Three aspects of child care
were measured from birth through 54 months: the
Participants
quantity of care, the quality of care, and the type of
Families were recruited through hospital visits to care.
mothers shortly after the birth of a child in 1991 in 10 Child-care quantity. Parents reported children’s
locations in the United States. During selected 24-hr hours of routine nonmaternal care during phone and
intervals, all women giving birth (n 5 8,986) were personal interviews conducted at 3-month intervals
screened for eligibility. From that group, 1,364 fam- through 36 months and at 4-month intervals there-
ilies completed a home interview when the infant after, as well as the type(s) of child care being used.
was 1-month-old and became the study participants. The hours spent in all settings were summed for each
Details of the sampling plan can be found in NICHD of the 17 intervals or ‘‘epochs’’ and parameterized on
ECCRN, 2005a). In terms of demographic charac- an hours-per-week basis. Individual measures of lev-
teristics, 26% of the mothers had no more than a high el and rate of change in quantity of care were
school education at the time of enrollment; 21% had computed as the individual intercepts and slopes
incomes no greater than 200% of the poverty level at from an unconditional hierarchical linear modeling
sixth grade; and 22% were minority (i.e., not non- (HLM) analysis of these 17 repeated measures. Age
Hispanic European American). was centered at the measurement midpoint, 27
As with any longitudinal study, not all families months; hence, the estimated intercept reflected that
participated in every wave of data collection. Rela- child’s hours per week at 27 months of age. In order
tively few families formally withdrew (N 5 291 to carry out the secondary analyses distinguishing
through sixth grade), but almost all children had at effects attributable to relative and nonrelative care,
least some missing data. Indeed, only 293 children the hours per week children spent in care proved by
had complete data on all predictors and outcomes fathers, romantic partner of mother, or grandparents
included in any analysis. Children were least likely (labeled relative care), and/or by other people be-
to be missing direct assessments and most likely to sides the mother (labeled nonrelative) was also
be missing teacher ratings. Children with complete computed. The hours per week in relative care and
Early Child Care 685

separately in nonrelative care were tallied for each symptoms assessed by the Center for Epidemiological
time point, and then the mean across time was Studies Depression (CESD) Scales reported by the
computed. mother at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months, determined by
Child-care type. For each epoch, each of the child’s HLM analyses. Composite parenting quality scores
care arrangements was classified as center, child-care were created by first averaging standardized ratings
home (any home-based care outside the child’s own of observed maternal sensitivity and of observed
home except care by grandparents), in-home care home environmental quality at 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54
(any caregiver in the child’s own home except father months and then estimating their intercept and
or grandparent), grandparent care, or father care. slope in HLM analyses. These control variables
The proportion of epochs in which the child received are described in detail in NICHD ECCRN (2002).
care in a center for at least 10 hr/week and the pro- Subsequent maternal CESD and income/needs ra-
portion of epochs in a child-care home for at least tios were included in the analyses as time-varying
10 hr/week were determined and were used as covariates.
variables to represent type of care. Primary-grades’ family covariate. Measures of fam-
Child-care quality. Observational assessments were ily demographic and psychological characteristics
conducted in the primary child-care arrangement at also were obtained when children were in kinder-
ages 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months. Quality was as- garten and in first, third, and fifth grades. These
sessed during two half-day visits scheduled within a were included as time-varying covariates in the
2-week interval at 6 – 36 months and one half-day HLM analyses of child outcomes. These factors were
visit at 54 months. Observers completed four 44-min presence of a husband/partner in the household, income-
cycles of the Observational Record of the Caregiving to-needs ratio, maternal depressive symptoms, and par-
Environment (ORCE) per child age through 36 enting quality.
months and two 44-min ORCE cycles at 54 months. Measures of parenting quality were collected every
Detailed descriptions of the ORCE assessments can 2 years using videotaped mother – child interactions
be found in NICHD ECCRN (2002), including coding involving 15-min semistructured tasks (NICHD
definitions, training procedures, and interobserver ECCRN, 2003a). At 54 months, the task included
agreement. Reliability exceeded .90 at 6 months, .86 completing a maze using an Etch-A-Sketch, building
at 15 months, .81 at 24 months, .80 at 36 months, and a series of identical towers from blocks of varying
.90 at 54 months. As with quantity, individual shapes and sizes, and playing together with six hand
measures of level and change in quality were esti- puppets. During the first-grade assessment, the
mated with an unconditional HLM analysis. Many interaction tasks included working together to draw
children were not in child care for at least three time a picture of a house and a tree using an Etch-
points, and therefore their slopes could not be com- A-Sketch (with the mother controlling one knob and
puted and preliminary analyses based on imputed the child the other), a patterned block activity using
data did not suggest that the quality slope contrib- colored blocks of different parquet shapes to fill in
uted to analysis models. Therefore, only the quality geometric frames, and a card game. These activities
intercept was included in the reported analyses. The provided a context for observing the mother’s
quality of relative and nonrelative care was also support for the child in activities that could be
computed for the purposes of carrying out the sec- frustrating but also an opportunity for fun together.
ondary analyses. A mean quality score was com- Two activities were used to assess maternal sensi-
puted for each setting. tivity in third and fifth grades. The first activity at
both measurement occasions was a discussion of
topics that were sources of disagreement between
Maternal, Child, and Family Controls
the mother and child and chosen by them for con-
Early childhood covariates. Measures of maternal, sideration from a list provided. The second activity
child, and family characteristics during infancy and was a planning task. In third grade, the dyad was
early childhood were collected and used as controls given the challenge of planning the route on a spe-
for possible selection bias: maternal education (in cially prepared map for completing 11 errands
years); the study child’s race and ethnicity; the pro- while doing as little back-tracking as possible; in
portion of (five measurement) epochs through 54 fifth grade, the task was to create a bungee jump
months in which the mother reported a husband/ for an egg, using a platform and a pair of nylon
partner was present; family income through 54 stockings (as the bungee cord) that would allow
months calculated as the mean income-to-needs ratio; the egg to fall within 2 in. of a table top and not
and the intercept and slope of maternal depressive break.
686 Belsky et al.

As was the case for earlier observational assess- Classroom Quality in the Primary Grades
ments, videotapes from all data collection sites were
Children’s classroom experiences were measured
shipped to the same central location for coding.
using the Classroom Observation System for First
Teams of three or four coders scored the mother –
Grade (NICHD ECCRN, 2004a), the Classroom Ob-
child interaction videotapes from each time period,
servation System for Third Grade (NICHD ECCRN,
with one or two members of a coding team carrying
2005c), and the Classroom Observation System for
over from one time period to the next. Coders were
Fifth Grade (NICHD ECCRN, 2004c). These obser-
blind as to other information about the families.
vations focused on the classroom as well as the
Coders received intensive training and supervision
specific study child and his or her classroom expe-
and typically met weekly or biweekly to recode tapes
riences. In first grade, two 44-min observations were
together as a group throughout the period of formal
conducted during the morning. In third and fifth
scoring. Complete operational and coding manuals
grades, classrooms were observed for eight 44-min
can be found at http://secc.rti.org/. Maternal sen-
cycles distributed across the school day. Three 7-
sitivity scores at each assessment age were the sum
point global ratings of the classroom environment
of the 7-point ratings of supportive presence, respect
were made at the end of each observation cycle:
for autonomy, and hostility (reversed). Cronbach as
overcontrol by teacher, and teacher’s emotional de-
for the sensitivity composite scores ranged from .80
tachment, teacher’s sensitivity to student needs.
to .85 and interrater reliabilities determined from
Observers from all 10 sites first trained on practice
intraclass correlations (Winer, 1971) based on a sec-
videotapes using a standardized detailed manual
ond coding of 19.5% (196/1,004) to 27% (271/987) of
and attended a centralized training workshop. All
the videotapes at the different ages ranged from .84
observers passed a videotaped reliability test in-
to .91.
volving six cases. The average reliability for the
The Home Observation for Measurement of the Envi-
teacher and classroom global ratings on the video-
ronment (HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 1984) was ad-
taped test was estimated at .60 using a correlation
ministered during home visits at 54 months and in
method and .69 using intraclass correlations (Winer,
third and fifth grades. The focus is on the child as a
1971). For more details on the derivation of these
recipient of inputs from objects, events, and trans-
composites and the individual scales of which they
actions occurring in connection with the family sur-
are composed, see NICHD ECCRN (2004a, 2005c).
roundings. Information is obtained during the
course of a home visit by means of observation and
semistructured interview. A centrally located system After-School Experience
of training was used for data collectors at each age.
Every 4 months, observers coded videotaped visits Mothers were interviewed by telephone in the fall
and the coding was compared with gold standard and spring of kindergarten and first, third, and fifth
codes. All observers were required to maintain a grades about the study children’s out-of-school care.
criterion of agreement with the master coder on 90% They were asked a series of questions about a
of the items. Cronbach as for the total score at each number of possible out-of-school care arrangements.
age exceeded .82. In the present study analyses, hours of nonparental
The HOME and maternal sensitivity ratings were out-of-school care arrangements (here named after-
standardized and averaged at each age to create a school hours) were obtained for each school year from
composite score, using an average of the 54-months the average across the spring and fall reports of the
and third-grade HOME to impute the missing first- total hours mothers reported across all nonparental
grade HOME scores. Together, these combined out-of-school care arrangements.
scores reflect parenting in two contexts: in the home
and during semistructured play. We have found this
Child Outcomes
composite parenting rating to be a strong and con-
sistent predictor of children’s cognitive and social Cognitive and social outcomes were assessed re-
competencies at earlier ages (ECCRN, 2002, 2003a, peatedly over time. Standardized cognitive-aca-
2005c). Two indexes of parenting quality (the inter- demic achievement tests were administered at 54
cept and slope) were created from the mean of the months and at the end of first, third, and fifth grades.
standardized scores at each age using HLM. The Caregiver/teacher reports of children’s behavior
composite parenting scores from 54 months through problems were collected at 54 months, kindergarten,
fifth grade were entered as time-varying concurrent and annually in first, second, third, fourth, fifth, and
controls in the second set of analyses. sixth grades; social skills from K through sixth grade;
Early Child Care 687

and emotional functioning and academic work hab- Behavior problems. The Child Behavior Checklist
its annually in first though sixth grade. Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach, 1991) was
Academic achievement. With respect to cognitive- used to evaluate problem behavior. The TRF lists 100
academic achievement, children were administered problem behaviors that generate two subscales: in-
four subtests from the Woodcock – Johnson Psycho- ternalizing problems (e.g., ‘‘too fearful and anxious’’)
Educational Battery – Revised: Letter – Word Identifi- and externalizing problems (e.g., ‘‘hits others,’’
cation (54 months and first grade), which assesses ‘‘disobedient at school,’’ ‘‘argues a lot’’). Achenbach
prereading skills in identifying isolated letters, and reports a test – retest reliability of .89, an interparent
words and Broad Reading (third and fifth grades), agreement of .70, and a stability of .71 over 2 years.
which adds assessment of passage comprehension to Raw scores were converted into standard T scores,
the assessment of identification of words; Applied based on normative data for children of the same age.
Problems, which measures skill in analyzing and Conflict with teacher. The Student – Teacher Rela-
solving practical problems in mathematics; and Pic- tionship Scale (STRS; Pianta, 2001) was completed by
ture Vocabulary, which measures children’s ability to caregivers/teachers. The STRS is a widely used in-
name objects depicted in a series of pictures. Items dicator of a teacher’s perceptions of the quality of his
are presented in order of increasing difficulty and are or her relationship with a specific child. In the cur-
scored 0 5 incorrect or no response, or 1 5 correct re- rent report, we focused on teacher – child conflict
sponse, with basal and ceiling levels established. (e.g., ‘‘dealing with this child drains my energy’’),
Typically, raw scores are converted to standard which was assessed by 7 items rated using 5-point
scores with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation Likert scales. Coefficient as for the conflict subscale
of 15, but for this study we relied upon W ability ranged from .88 to .91 across grades.
scores so that change over time could be more easily Work habits. Teachers completed a 19-item mock
documented. The W ability scores are transform- report card. This questionnaire includes 6 items ad-
ations of the Rasch raw ability scores designed to dressing the child’s work habits. The items, each rated
eliminate the need for decimal fractions and negative on a 5-point scale (1 5 very poor to 5 5 very good),
values. The overall Woodcock – Johnson – Revised were taken from Madison (Wisconsin) Metropolitan
battery of tests has been standardized on a nationally School District report cards. The 6 items include
representative sample from 24 months to 95 years of ‘‘follows classroom procedures,’’ ‘‘works well inde-
age. Internal-consistency reliability for the full bat- pendently,’’ ‘‘works neatly and carefully,’’ ‘‘uses time
tery of subscales ranged from .94 to .98, with test – wisely,’’ ‘‘completes work promptly,’’ and ‘‘keeps
retest reliability ranging from .80 to .87. Moreover, material organized.’’ Scores were based on the mean
the subtests used here correlated substantially with of the items at each age. Coefficient as were high,
other cognitive assessments (McGrew, Werder, & ranging from .94 to .95.
Woodcock, 1991) and have been found in previous Socioemotional functioning. Teacher-reported so-
work with the current sample to relate strongly to cial – emotional functioning was obtained from the
aspects of experience presumed to influence aca- mock report cards. The 7 items included that ad-
demic achievement, most notably quality of parent- dressed social – emotional functioning came from the
ing, (NICHD ECCRN, 2005b). Teacher Checklist of Peer Relations (Coie & Dodge,
Social skills. The Social Skills Questionnaire from 1988) and rated on a 5-point scale (1 5 very poor to
the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham & 5 5 very good). Items addressed the children’s social
Elliott, 1990) was used to assess social competence skillfulness with peers, such as ‘‘generates good
and social skills. This instrument is composed of 38 quality solutions to interpersonal problems’’ and ‘‘is
items describing child behavior, each rated on a 3- aware of the effects of his/her behavior on others.’’
point scale reflecting how often the child exhibited Cronbach as ranged from .94 to .95.
each behavior. Items are grouped into four areas:
cooperation (e.g., ‘‘keeps room neat and clean with-
Data Analysis Plan
out being reminded’’), assertion (e.g., ‘‘makes friends
easily’’), responsibility (e.g., ‘‘asks permission before Data analysis focused on testing the long-term
using someone else’s property’’), and self-control associations between child-care experiences during
(e.g., ‘‘controls temper when arguing with other the first 4 12 years and children’s academic and social
children’’). The total score used in this report repre- development from that age through the spring of
sents the sum of all 38 items, with higher scores re- sixth grade. HLM (Bryk & Raudenbush, 2002; Singer
flecting higher levels of perceived social skills (as & Willett, 2003) were fitted to estimate individual
range from .86 to .94). and group linear and quadratic growth curves. The
688 Belsky et al.

models included both preschool and concurrent family, and child-care measures, there was sufficient
family and child-care/school experiences. Individu- information in our data to estimate missing data
al intercepts and linear slopes with respect to age accurately (rather than rely on simple mean substi-
were estimated as correlated random effects for each tution). Schafer’s (1997; Schafer & Graham, 2002)
child for each outcome. These individual-level pa- recommended procedure, an iterative E-M algo-
rameters were related to predictors of interest and rithm, was used. Missing values for each variable are
covariates. The predictors of primary interest were estimated iteratively using a logistic or multiple re-
the five indexes of child-care experiences (across the gression from all the other variables using the data
period 3 – 54 months): proportion of 3 – 4 month ep- for all individuals with observed values on that
ochs in center-based child care for at least 10 hr/ variable, and random variability is added as the
week, proportion of 3 – 4 month epochs in a child- missing data are predicted. The process is repeated
care home for at least 10 hr/week, the hours per for each variable until the differences in predicted
week intercept (estimated from HLM analyses in values across iterations are miniscule. Five data sets
which the intercept was set at 27 months, reflecting were created in which all observed data are repre-
the midpoint between 3 and 54 months), the hours sented and missing data are estimated. Conse-
per week slope (estimated linear change over time in quently, analyses were conducted five times, using
hours per week), and the quality intercept (estimated each of the five imputation data sets. The results
quality of care at 27 months). The preschool time- of these analyses were combined using the recom-
invariant covariates included site, child ethnicity, mended procedures of Schafer (1997) to ensure that
child gender, maternal education, mean income-to- variability in imputed values across the data sets as
needs ratio between 6 and 54 months, parenting well as variability in the variables within the data set
intercept, and slope from 6 to 54 months, maternal were considered. The test statistics and regression
depressive symptoms intercept and slope from 6 to coefficients were averaged across the five analyses,
54 months. The concurrent time-varying covariates and the standard errors for the coefficients were
from 54 months through sixth grade included in- combined by combining within- and between-model
come-to-needs ratio, parenting, maternal depression, variability.
observed school classroom quality, and hours per Effect sizes were computed when child-care
week of after-school care (set to 0 for 54 months). variables showed a significant association with child
Several modeling decisions were made. All child- outcome trajectories. Effect sizes were computed to
care predictor variables and covariates were centered show the anticipated difference in standard devi-
at the sample mean to enhance interpretation of ation units of the outcome measure between children
main effects. Age was centered at the mean age for who had child-care experiences that differed by one
spring of the final assessment periodFfifth grade standard deviation (for details, see NICHD ECCRN
for academic achievement and sixth grade for & Duncan, 2003). The effect sizes were computed as
teacher ratings. Thus, the main effect coefficient for the product of the estimated child-care coefficient
each child-care predictor indicates the extent to and the standard deviation for the child-care index
which that variable was related to the outcome in the divided by the standard deviation for the outcome
spring of either fifth or sixth grade, that is, the most measure, and can be interpreted somewhat like a
recent time of measurement for that outcome. When correlation. For example, the standard deviation for
the child-care predictors showed interactions with our child-care quality measure was .23; therefore, the
age, we estimated coefficients for that child-care effect size compared predicted outcome scores for
variable at each of the ages at which the outcome was children whose child care differed in quality by .23
measured (Aiken & West, 1991). This approach al- points on the ORCE measure of child-care quality.
lows for a representation of interactions between
age and continuous variables in a way that illus-
Results
trates findings from the analysis that produced those
interactions.
Descriptive Analyses
Missing data occurred in this longitudinal project
due to attrition and failure to complete all assess- The descriptive statistics for all child-care and
ments. Missing data were imputed using multiple family measures from the early childhood period are
imputation (Rubin, 1987; Schafer, 1997; Schafer & shown in Table 1 and for the academic and social
Graham, 2002) under the assumption that missing outcomes and concurrent school and family charac-
data were ignorably missing. That is, given our teristics are shown in Table 2. Correlations among
many longitudinal measures on demographic, child, the child-care variables, the family, and school
Early Child Care 689

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics: Early Childhood Child Care and Family Measures

Child-Care Variables N Mean SD Minimum Maximum

Overall
Proportion of timeaFcenter care: 1 – 54 months 1,214 0.21 0.26 0 0.94
Proportion of timeaFchild care home: 1 – 54 months 1,214 0.19 0.28 0 1.00
Hours per week 1 – 54 months: intercept 1,268 24.90 16.00 0.77 61.33
Hours per week 1 – 54 months: linear change 1,268 0.23 0.31 1.05 1.24
ORCE quality total 6 – 54 months: intercept 1,134 2.81 .23 2.07 3.44
In care by relatives
Hours per week: 1 – 54 months 1,214 7.37 9.83 0 53.71
Mean ORCE quality total: 6 – 54 months 426 2.99 0.50 1.55 3.95
In care by nonrelatives
Hours per week of care: 1 – 54 months 1,214 16.48 14.16 0 52.76
Mean ORCE quality total: 6 – 54 months 1,005 2.90 0.45 1.35 4.00
Family variables
Mother’s education 1,363 14.23 2.51 7 21
Proportion of timeb Partner in HH: 1 – 54 months 1,305 0.84 0.32 0 1
Income/poverty threshold: mean 6 – 54 months 1,302 3.60 2.85 0.15 27.36
Maternal depression (CESD) 6 – 54 months: intercept 1,304 9.35 5.49 1.39 33.57
Maternal depression (CESD) 6 – 54 months: linear change 1,260 0.19 0.64 3.10 4.05
Parenting 6 – 54 months: intercept 1,306 0.03 0.67 3.19 1.27
Parenting 6 – 54 months: linear change 1,261 0.01 0.07 0.32 0.20

Note. CESD 5 Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression; ORCE 5 Observational Record of the Caregiving Environment.
a
Proportion of 17 measurement epochs.
b
Proportion of 5 measurement epochs.

covariates, and the child outcomes that are shown in Primary Longitudinal Prediction Analyses
Table 3 indicate that most of the child-care variables
were modestly correlated. Stronger correlations ap- Two sets of hierarchical linear model analyses of
peared between the amount and type of child care, the child outcomes from 54 months through sixth
indicating that children who spent more hours per grade were conducted. The first primary set of
week in care (hours intercept) were more likely to analyses included the five child-care measures that
have spent more months in either a center or a child- reflected the type, quantity, and quality of care dur-
care home. The correlations between quantity and ing early childhood. In addition, all analyses in-
quality of care in relative and nonrelative care were cluded site and selected child and family measures
modestly correlated. As children spent more time in from early childhood as covariates to control for
nonrelative care, the quantity and quality of care in possible selection factors. The second set of analyses
nonrelative care were somewhat more strongly cor- examined the extent to which care by relative and
related with overall measures of quantity and quality nonrelatives predicted trajectories, controlling for
than were quantity and quality in nonrelative care. the same covariates; the results of these analyses are
As shown in Table 4, the three academic measures described in the secondary analysis subsection.
and the five teacher ratings were strongly correlated. Table 6 presents the primary results from first set
Finally, Table 5 shows the correlations between the of HLM analyses after aggregating the results across
child-care variables and child outcomes at the final the analyses of the five imputation data sets. Owing
age at which all outcomes were collected. Most of to the primary focus of this report on effects of child
these correlations were modest in magnitude. It ap- care and in the interests of space, Table 6 presents the
peared that quality of relative care was a somewhat coefficients for the main effect and interaction with
stronger positive correlate of academic outcomes age for each of the five selected child-care indexes. In
than quality of nonrelative care, but that quantity of the bottom subsection of the table, coefficients per-
nonrelative care was a somewhat stronger negative taining to parenting are presented to provide a
correlate of teacher rating of behavior between 54 comparison for the coefficients for the child-care
months and in first grade than at subsequent ages. variables because it is widely accepted that parenting
690 Belsky et al.

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics: Longitudinal Child Outcomes and Family Measures from 54 months to Grade 6

Age at Assessment

54 K G1 G2 G3 G4 G5 G6

Child Outcomes
WJ Letter Word/Broad Reading W score
N 1,056 1,025 1,011 993
Mean 369.4 452.6 494.6 507.6
SD 21.41 23.99 15.75 14.12
WJ Applied Problems/Broad Math W score
N 1,053 1,023 1,012 993
Mean 424.7 470.0 493.5 510.5
SD 19.27 15.54 12.76 13.04
WJ Picture Vocabulary W score
N 1,060 1,020 1,014 992
Mean 459.5 483.9 496.9 505.8
SD 14.09 12.27 11.51 12.08
Teacher report CBCL externalizing
N 714 1,004 1,007 921 982 914 927 855
Mean 50.09 49.72 50.68 50.51 51.51 50.46 50.96 50.16
SD 9.61 8.86 8.72 8.91 9.36 9.09 9.15 9.12

Teacher report SSRS total


N 993 1,000 913 975 906 921 842
Mean 103.5 103.2 105.1 102.2 102.4 102.8 102.9
SD 14.05 13.64 14.52 14.48 13.94 14.44 14.26
Teacher report STRS Conflict
N 716 1,006 1,006 935 978 915 930 857
Mean 18.88 10.60 10.92 10.94 11.62 11.14 11.44 11.07
SD 6.66 5.36 5.17 5.41 6.03 5.73 5.74 5.64
Teacher report ECLS Social – emotional evaluation
N 1,005 918 993 932 940 866
Mean 3.52 3.62 3.55 3.59 3.66 3.65
SD 0.87 0.96 0.90 0.90 0.89 0.88
Teacher report work habits evaluation
N 1,006 920 992 930 941 871
Mean 3.55 3.63 3.58 3.64 3.70 3.69
SD 1.05 1.11 1.08 1.07 1.04 1.12

Family and School Covariates


Income/poverty threshold
N 1,073 1,027 982 982 985 996 979
Mean 3.59 3.49 3.95 4.39 4.50 4.53 4.54
SD 3.17 2.69 3.03 3.77 3.88 4.06 4.15
Maternal depression: CESD score
N 1,077 1,099 1,009 1,103 1,026 1,077 1,019 1,023
Mean 9.83 9.30 8.39 8.78 9.08 8.91 8.73 8.96
SD 8.70 7.72 8.47 7.84 8.85 7.78 8.62 8.82
Parenting
N 1,069 1,142 1,139 1,139 1,032 1,078 1,038 1,038
Mean 0.00 0.02 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.01
SD 0.85 0.81 0.86 0.82 0.86 0.79 0.86 0.86
Partner in home
N 1,084 1,059 1,034 1,123 1,076 1,059 1,030 1,025
Proportion 0.83 0.83 0.82 0.81 0.81 0.80 0.81 0.81
Early Child Care 691

Table 2. (Contd)

Age at Assessment

54 K G1 G2 G3 G4 G5 G6

Classroom quality
N 854 966 971 955
Mean 2.98 3.43 3.17 3.21
SD 0.56 0.47 0.48 0.36
Hours in after-school care
N 1,071 1,040 1,014 1,054 1,046 1,047 1,047
Mean 9.28 6.54 6.35 5.65 5.81 6.08 6.08
SD 9.47 5.98 5.78 5.33 5.42 5.26 5.26

Note. CESD 5 Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression; SSRS 5 Social Skills Questionnaire from the Social Skills Rating System;
STRS 5 Student – Teacher Relationship Scale.

is strongly linked to children’s development (NI- the measures collected during early childhood and
CHD ECCRN, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c). The main effect concurrently.
coefficients represent the estimated associations be-
tween child care and child outcomes at the final as-
Child-Care Quality
sessment (fifth grade for the academic outcomes,
sixth grade for teacher questionnaires). The age The first rows in Table 6 present the results re-
interaction coefficients indicate the extent to which garding child-care quality. The quality intercept (es-
the associations between child care and child out- timated quality at 27 months) was significantly and
comes change over time. Coefficients that were positively related to one outcome, vocabulary (pic-
deemed significant (po.05) are given in bold. In ture vocabulary): Children who had experienced
addition, effect sizes were estimated to describe the higher quality care had higher vocabulary scores in
association between each select child-care index and fifth grade (B 5 3.15, po.05; d 5 .06). This association
that child outcome at most assessment ages. Coeffi- did not change reliably over time; that is, the Age 
cients for covariates are not presented (except, for Child-Care Quality interaction was statistically non-
comparative purposes, in the case of parenting) to significant. Child-care quality became a significantly
preserve space and because of the difficulty in in- weaker predictor of reading skills over time (B 5
terpreting them due to the high correlations between 1.08, po.05). Children who experienced higher

Table 3
Correlations Among Child Care Variables

% CC Hours per Hours per CC quality Relative Nonrelative


home week intercept week slope intercept hours Quality hours Quality

Overall
% Centera .17 .40 .11 .21 .12 .05 .45 .16
% CC homea .45 .08 .05 .12 .01 .51 .10
Hours intercept .01 .10 .31 .02 .71 .21
Hours slope .07 .07 .07 .07 .05
Quality intercept .09 .53 .15 .55
Relative care
Hours mean .11 .26 .07
Quality mean .08 .10
Nonrelative care
Hours mean .17

Note. aProportion of 17 measurement epochs.


po.05, po.01, po.005.
692 Belsky et al.

Table 4
Correlations Among Outcome Measures in Fifth Grade

WJ-R WJ-R TRF CTRS SSRS social ECLS social/ Work


WJ-R reading math vocabulary external conflict kkills emotionl habits

Reading .68 .67 .20 .19 .29 .35 .36


Math .56 .22 .21 .34 .37 .40
Vocabulary .19 .18 .24 .28 .25
Externalizing .76 .60 .65 .55
Conflict .62 .65 .56
Social skills .70 .68
Social – emotional Work habits .70

Note. SSRS 5 Social Skills Rating System; TRF 5 Child Behavior Checklist Teacher Report Form.
po.05, po.01, po.005.

quality care had significantly higher reading scores care analyses carried out at earlier ages (NICHD
at 54 months (B 5 6.47, po.05; d 5 .06), but this as- Early Child Care Research Network, 2005a, 2005b,
sociation was no longer significant by first grade or 2006). These follow-up analyses focused on family
thereafter, becoming quite small by fifth grade and school characteristics and involved two steps:
(B 5 0.01, p4.05; d 5 .001). first identifying which such variables were correlat-
ed with change in quantity of child care and then
testing whether these identified variables accounted
Quantity of Child Care
for the association between change in quantity of
The next set of rows display the coefficients and child care and vocabulary development. Whereas
effect sizes associated with the intercept and slope school characteristics proved to be unrelated to
from the unconditional HLM analysis of hours of change in child care quantity during early child-
child care (i.e., the expected hours of care at 27 hood, both the age of entry into child care, w2(8,
months and linear change in hours from 1 to 54 n 5 1,364) 5 296, po.001, and whether the family
months, respectively). The hours intercept became was poor during early childhood (indicated by mean
a statistically significant weaker predictor of teach- of 6 – 54 months income/needso2.0) w2(2,
er ratings of externalizing problems (B 5 .012, n 5 1,364) 5 39, po.001, were significantly related.
po.001) and teacher – child conflict (B 5 .009, When these two variables were included in the
po.001) over time, eventually turning nonsignificant. prediction model (as main effects and an inter-
The association between the hours per week of care in action), linear change in hours no longer significantly
early childhood and teacher ratings of externalizing related to vocabulary scores. Instead, the interaction
problems ranged from a significant B 5 .065 (po.001; between age at entry to child care and whether the
d 5 .11,) at 54 months to a nonsignificant B 5 .018 family was poor (median income/needso2) indi-
(p4.05; d 5 .03) at sixth grade. Similarly, the asso- cated that whereas there was no significant associa-
ciation between hours per week of care and teacher tion between entry age and vocabulary among
ratings of conflict ranged from a significant B 5 .052 children whose families were not poor, among low-
(po.001; d 5 .12) at 54 months to a nonsignificant income children vocabulary scores varied as a
B 5 .008 (p4.05; d 5 .02) at sixth grade.‘ function of age of entry to care: Scores were higher if
The hours’ slope emerged as a significant pre- (poor) children entered child care before 3 months of
dictor of one outcome. This previously undetected age or after 9 months of age and lower if they entered
association indicated that children whose hours of between 3 and 9 months.
child care increased more over time (i.e., from 3 to 54
months) had significantly lower vocabulary scores in
Type of Child Care
fifth grade (B 5 2.67, po.05; d 5 .07). This
‘‘sleeper effect’’ result was explored in subsequent The next set of rows presents the coefficients asso-
analyses to determine whether it might be an artifact ciated with the proportion of epochs in which the
of other factors/processes because no link between mother reported that the child attended a center or was
quantity of child care and cognitive – language de- in a child-care home. Only one association was statis-
velopment had heretofore been detected in child- tically significant: Teachers reported more problem
Early Child Care 693

Table 5
Correlations Between Child Care Variables and Longitudinal Child Outcomes

Nonmaternal care Relative care Nonrelative care Parents

% Child Hours Hours Hours Hours


% care per week per week Quality per per Parenting
Centera homea intercept slope intercpt week Quality week Quality intercpt

WJ-R reading 54 months .09 .01 .08 .05 .23 .01 .30 .07 .14 .44
G1 .08 .00 .06 .09 .16 .02 .20 .08 .11 .34
G3 .06 .02 .07 .09 .19 .01 .25 .06 .11 .45
G5 .03 .02 .05 .10 .19 .02 .23 .05 .13 .48
WJ-R math 54 months .07 .05 .07 .06 .21 .09 .21 .09 .20 .52
G1 .10 .01 .08 .06 .19 .08 .26 .13 .13 .43
G3 .07 .02 .09 .06 .18 .04 .24 .10 .14 .40
G5 .04 .00 .07 .09 .18 .05 .24 .08 .16 .45
WJ vocabulary 54 months .06 .02 .06 .11 .24 .06 .30 .07 .20 .51
G1 .05 .01 .04 .09 .22 .01 .29 .04 .15 .50
G3 .06 .07 .09 .11 .23 .00 .29 .08 .14 .48
G5 .04 .06 .06 .16 .22 .03 .26 .07 .15 .48
T externalizing 54 months .16 .04 .22 .07 .14 .03 .15 .21 .16 .26
G1 .11 .08 .14 .05 .09 .01 .06 .17 .11 .30
G3 .10 .00 .10 .11 .17 .01 .20 .10 .15 .37
G5 .04 .02 .03 .05 .15 .00 .21 .05 .11 .34
G6 .04 .03 .07 .06 .10 .00 .21 .05 .11 .34
STRS conflict 54 months .15 .02 .18 .05 .14 .01 .12 .19 .16 .16
G1 .09 .11 .15 .03 .12 .02 .09 .18 .11 .22
G3 .04 .00 .04 .10 .15 .01 .19 .06 .14 .32
G5 .03 .01 .02 .07 .10 .03 .16 .02 .09 .35
G6 .00 .02 .03 .00 .09 .02 .15 .05 .07 .29
SSRS social skills 54 months .04 .03 .07 .11 .13 .04 .14 .03 .18 .27
G1 .07 .02 .07 .07 .13 .04 .22 .06 .12 .33
G3 .01 .03 .00 .08 .15 .03 .19 .01 .12 .32
G5 .00 .05 .03 .08 .15 .06 .23 .06 .10 .36
G6 .05 .04 .01 .09 .11 .05 .22 .04 .07 .35
ECLS Social – emotional
G1 .06 .05 .08 .07 .15 .02 .12 .09 .11 .36
G3 .03 .02 .02 .12 .17 .03 .22 .00 .13 .40
G5 .02 .02 .01 .08 .17 .04 .22 .02 .11 .40
G6 .05 .00 .00 .07 .13 .03 .23 .02 .11 .40
Work habits G1 .05 .00 .04 .08 .13 .06 .14 .03 .11 .37
G3 .00 .02 .01 .06 .17 .08 .16 .01 .13 .40
G5 .03 .04 .05 .07 .14 .04 .18 .06 .10 .42
G6 .05 .04 .03 .05 .17 .04 .23 .04 .14 .41

Note. SSRS 5 Social Skills Questionnaire from the Social Skills Rating System; STRS 5 Student – Teacher Relationship Scale.
a
Proportion of 17 measurement epochs.
po.05, po.01, po.001.

behaviors for children who spent more time in centers all the developmental outcomes and much more
(B 5 2.85; po.01; effect size d 5 .08). This association strongly than did any of the child-care predictors.
did not change reliably over time; the Age  Center Higher levels of parenting quality (i.e., intercept)
care interaction was not statistically significant. predicted greater tested reading, math, and vocabu-
lary achievement in fifth grade and lower levels of
teacher-rated externalizing problems and conflict
Parenting Quality
and higher levels of social skills, social – emotional
In marked contrast to the child-care effects just functioning, and work habits in sixth grade.
described, parenting quality significantly predicted The magnitude of parenting effects on math and
694
Table 6
HLM Analysis of Child Outcomes From 54 Months to Grade 6: Coefficients Predicting Outcomes from Child Care Experiences All Care

Direct assessment Teacher report

WJ-R WJ-R WJ-R Social Social – Work


reading math vocabulary Externalizing skills Conflict emotional habits
Belsky et al.

CC quality B (SE) 0.01 (2.24) 1.35 (1.86) 3.15 (1.58) 0.48 (1.24) 1.31 (2.29) 0.10 (0.67) 0.10 (0.11) 0.17 (0.17)
CC Quality  Age B (SE) 1.08 (0.41) 0.47 (0.34) 0.14 (0.29) 0.12 (0.21) 0.11 (0.31) 0.05 (0.12) 0.02 (0.03) 0.05 (0.04)
54 months ES 0.06 0.05 0.06 0.01 0.01 0.01
G1 ES 0.04 0.04 0.06 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.00 0.01
G3 ES 0.03 0.04 0.06 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01
G5 ES 0.00 0.02 0.06 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.03
G6 ES 0.01 0.02 0.00 0.02 0.03
CC hours intercept B (SE) 0.03 (0.04) 0.05 (0.03) 0.01 (0.03) 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.03) 0.01 (0.01) 0.001 (0.002) 0.000 (0.002)
CC Hours  Age B (SE) 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 0.001 (0.004) 0.012(0.003) 0.006 (0.005) 0.009(0.002) 0.000 (0.001) 0.001 (0.001)
54 months ES 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.11 0.07 0.12
G1 ES 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.07 0.05 0.11 0.04 0.07
G3 ES 0.02 0.04 0.01 0.03 0.04 0.05 0.03 0.04
G5 ES 0.03 0.06 0.01 0.01 0.03 0.00 0.02 0.02
G6 ES 0.03 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.00
CC hours slope B (SE) 1.70 (1.54) 0.94 (1.16) 2.67 (1.05) 0.76 (0.83) 1.18 (1.20) 0.09 (0.60) 0.03 (0.07) 0.06 (0.10)
CC Hours  Age B (SE) 0.10 (0.31) 0.24 (0.24) 0.36 (0.20) 0.00 (0.14) 0.01 (0.19) 0.09(0.12) 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02)
54 months ES 0.02 0.01 0.01 0.03 0.03 0.03
G1 ES 0.02 0.00 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.02 0.04 0.02
G3 ES 0.03 0.01 0.05 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.03 0.00
G5 ES 0.04 0.02 0.07 0.03 0.02 0.00 0.02 0.01
G6 ES 0.03 0.03 0.00 0.01 0.02
Prop.Center Carea B (SE) 0.94 (2.08) 0.80 (1.80) 1.41 (1.52) 2.87 (1.35) 0.85 (2.02) 0.60 (0.73) 0.05 (0.14) 0.03 (0.16)
Prop.Centera  Age B (SE) 0.77 (0.41) 0.60 (0.34) 0.19 (0.28) 0.22 (0.23) 0.28 (0.32) 0.21 (0.13) 0.03 (0.03) 0.03 (0.04)
54 months ES 0.04 0.04 0.01 0.12 0.02 0.08
G1 ES 0.02 0.03 0.01 0.12 0.01 0.08 0.07 0.03
G3 ES 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.10 0.00 0.05 0.05 0.01
G5 ES 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.09 0.01 0.04 0.02 0.00
G6 ES 0.08 0.02 0.03 0.01 0.01
Prop. child care homea B (SE) 0.20 (2.28) 0.82 (1.75) 1.82 (1.51) 1.08 (1.20) 1.85 (1.62) 0.15 (0.67) 0.05 (0.10) 0.15 (0.12)
Prop. Child Care B (SE) 0.08 (0.40) 0.56 (.36) 0.32 (0.23) 0.02 (0.19) 0.05 (0.26) 0.07 (0.12) 0.02 (0.03) 0.02 (0.03)
Homea  Age
54 months ES 0.003 0.04 0.00 0.03 0.04 0.03
G1 ES 0.001 0.02 0.01 0.04 0.04 0.03 0.01 0.02
G3 ES 0.001 0.01 0.03 0.03 0.04 0.02 0.00 0.03
G5 ES 0.004 0.02 0.04 0.03 0.04 0.01 0.01 0.04
G6 0.03 0.04 0.01 0.02 0.04
Early Child Care 695

Note. Model includes as time-invariant covariates: site, gender, ethnicity, maternal education, proportion time mother had partner in household, 6 – 54-month maternal depression
intercept and slope, and as time-varying covariates: concurrent measures of income/needs, partner in household, parenting, maternal depression, classroom quality, and after-school
0.30(0.07)
vocabulary achievement weakened over time, as re-
0.01 (0.02)
vealed by a significant interaction with age, but
nevertheless remained significant even at the final
time of measurement.
0.21
0.20
0.19
0.18
Secondary Analyses
0.33(0.07)
0.02 (0.02)

Two sets of secondary analyses were conducted.


The first was based on the possibilities (a) that pre-
0.19
0.21
0.23
0.24

viously detected effects of center-based child care


and hours in any child care on externalizing prob-
lems might be most likely to emerge for boys and (b)
1.27 (0.52)
0.11 (0.09)

that previously detected effects of child-care quality


on cognitive – academic achievement might be most
0.05
0.09
0.10
0.13
0.15

likely to emerge for children from the most eco-


nomically disadvantaged families (i.e., income-to-
needs ratioo2.0). Models predicting externalizing
problems and academic achievement were revised to
3.72 (0.93)

include relevant interaction terms. As in previous


0.00 (0.18)

analyses on children at younger ages (NICHD


0.18
0.18
0.17
0.17
0.17

ECCRN, 2003a, 2005a, 2005b), no evidence emerged


to indicate that that gender or income moderated the
reported results.
2.63(0.63)

A final set of secondary analyses focused on


0.15 (0.13)

whether care was provided by close relatives or not,


based on the results of a re-analysis of the 54-month
0.14
0.15
0.18
0.19
0.11

NICHD SECC data carried out by van IJzendoorn et


al. (2004) showing that the effect of time spent in any
kind of child care on externalizing problems report-
0.37 (0.15)
4.71 (0.70)

ed by the NICHD Early Child Care Research Net-


work (2003a, 2003b, 2003c) was principally a function
of time spent in nonrelative care. This led to the ex-
0.32
0.33
0.31
0.25

pectation that distinguishing between nonmaternal


care by relatives (i.e., fathers, grandparents) and by
1.16(0.19)

nonrelatives would reproduce already reported re-


2.80 (1.06)

sults pertaining to social functioning, but principally


in the case of care by nonrelatives rather than by
0.33
0.31
0.26
0.14

relatives. To address this issue, the hours per week of


care provided by a father or grandparent and the
average quality of such care were distinguished from
5.18(1.04)
0.62 (0.35)

the amount and quality of care provided by anyone


else and regression analyses were run predicting all
0.27
0.21
0.27
0.24

Proportion of 17 measurement epochs.

the outcomes using measures of hours and quality of


care separately for the two groups of care providers
B (SE)
B (SE)

(i.e., relatives, nonrelatives).


ES
ES
ES
ES

po.05, po.01, po.001.

The results displayed in Table 7 of effects attrib-


utable to each kind of care (i.e., top: by relatives;
Parenting Quality  Age

bottom: by nonrelatives) reveal a pattern of findings


similar to those that emerged in the primary analyses
Parenting quality

(see Table 6) and, in general, consistent with those


54 months

reported by van IJzendorn et al. (2004), in that sig-


nificant effects emerged principally, even if not ex-
hours.

clusively, in the case of care by nonrelatives. More


G1
G3
G5
G6

specifically, hours per week of nonrelative care in-


a
696 Belsky et al.

.0014 (.001)
teracted with time such that between 54 months and

.000 (.004)
.001 (.001)

.001 (.002)
sixth grade the amount of nonrelative care became
habits
Work

.05 (.07)
.00 (.02)

.03 (.06)
.02 (.02)
less predictive of teacher ratings of externalizing
problems (B 5 .017, SE 5 .005, po.001) and conflict
(B 5 .010, SE 5 .003, po.001), and more predictive
of social – emotional adjustment (B 5 .0013, SE 5

.0013(.001)
.0006, po.05) and work habits (B 5 .0014, SE 5 .0005,
.002 (.003)
.000 (.001)

.000 (.002)
emotional
HLM Analysis of Child Outcomes From 54 Months to Grade 6: Coefficients Predicting Outcomes From Child Care Experiences Comparing Relative and Nonrelative Care

po.01). Finally, hours of nonrelative care was a


Social –

.03 (.06)
.00 (.02)

.03 (.05)
.01 (.02)
modest positive predictor of math skills (B 5 .07,
SE 5 .03, po.05), whereas hours of relative care was
a significantly weaker predictor of teacher ratings of
externalizing problems over time (B 5 .014, SE 5
.014 (.005)
Teacher report

.005, po.01). These results were viewed as buttress-


.005 (.004)
Conflict

ing the findings reported above because hours of


.01 (.02)

.15 (.62)
.00 (.12)

.00 (.01)

.08 (.37)
.05 (.07)

care by both relatives and nonrelatives were related


to ratings of problems by the child’s teachers and in
all cases became nonsignificant by the time the child
was in sixth grade.
(0.04)
(0.01)
(1.10)
(0.23)

(0.03)
(0.01)
(1.04)
(0.16)
Social
skills

0.01
0.01
1.86
0.03

0.01
0.01
0.68
0.16

Discussion
This paper represents the latest installment in the
ongoing study of relations between experiences in
.017 (.005)
.014 (.005)
Externalizing

child care in the first 54 months of life and child


development using data gathered in the NICHD
.04 (.03)

.22 (.84)
.12 (.14)

.02 (.02)

.47 (.66)
.05 (.11)

SECCYD. Four issues were addressed in this report:


(1) whether associations linking child functioning
with child-care quality, quantity, and type detected
before school entry and in first through third grade
0.009 (0.008)

0.004 (0.005)
0.11 (0.05)

continued to be evident in fifth and sixth grade; (2)


vocabulary

1.07 (1.44)
0.01 (0.20)

0.04 (0.03)

0.59 (0.95)
0.18 (0.17)

whether associations between child-care experiences


WJ-R

and child development dissipated over time; (3)


whether new relations emerged between child care
and child development (i.e., sleeper effects); and (4)
Direct assessment

how relations between child care and child devel-


0.002 (0.006)
0.07 (0.03)

opment compared, strengthwise, with linkages be-


(0.05)
(0.01)
(1.04)
(0.17)

1.12 (1.09)
0.26 (0.22)
WJ-R
math

tween parenting quality and child development. In


0.09
0.02
0.87
0.13

this latest installment at the end of fifth grade (for


cognitive and academic outcomes) and sixth grades
(for social and behavioral outcomes), we found evi-
dence of all three patterns of relations between child
.005 (0.008)

.001 (0.006)
.050 (0.05)

.24 (1.59)
.14 (0.26)

.01 (0.03)

.58 (1.12)
.07 (0.18)
reading

Note. HLM 5 hierarchical linear modeling.

care and child development just mentioned, as well


WJ-R

as evidence that parenting quality proved to be a far


stronger and more consistent predictor of tested
achievement and teacher-reported social functioning
po.05, po.01, po.001.

than was child-care experience.


(SE)
(SE)
(SE)
(SE)

(SE)
(SE)
(SE)
(SE)

Two predictive associations involving child care


B
B
B
B

B
B
B
B

first detected before school entry that were main-


tained through third grade remained statistically
Quality  Age

Quality  Age
Nonrelative care
Hours  Age

Hours  Age

significant in fifth and sixth grade; each is discussed


Relative care

in turn. First, children who experienced higher quality


Quality

Quality

early child care (of any kind) displayed somewhat better


Hours

Hours
Table 7

vocabulary scores in fifth grade than did children who


experienced poorer quality care. This potentially en-
Early Child Care 697

during ‘‘effect’’ of child-care quality on vocabulary is the primary and secondary analyses suggest that not
consistent with other evidence indicating that chil- only does it matter whether care is provided by
dren’s early experience matters to their language relatives or nonrelatives, but whether nonrelative
development. Perhaps the best example comes from care takes the specific form of center-based care.
Hart and Risley’s (1995) work tracking 42 families for Consider first in this regard that the data presented
9 years and showing that one of the strongest pre- in Table 7 indicate that previously reported findings
dictors of children’s vocabulary was adult talk, in- linking time in any kind of care with teacher – child
cluding talk in child-care settings. In fact, Hart and conflict, social – emotional competence, and aca-
Risley (1995) concluded that community child care demic work habits (NICHD Early Child Care Re-
can serve as an effective intervention for low-income search Network, 2005a, 2005b) were exclusively a
children who often do not experience a rich verbal function of time in nonrelative careFin that no main
environment in the home. Because vocabulary is one or age-moderated effect of hours of relative care
of the best predictors of reading (National Reading proved significant for these outcomes, only age-
Panel, 2000), the long-term relations that emerged in moderated effects of nonrelative care (i.e., bottom of
the current work between child-care quality and Table 7). The fact, however, that all the age-moder-
vocabularyFfor all children (i.e., not just low-in- ated effects of time in any kind of nonrelative care
come ones)Fmay have important implications for became insignificant by the time children were in
education policy. sixth grade, whereas the effect of center care on ex-
The second enduring link between early child care ternalizing problems remained significantFand did
and child development detected in this inquiry in- not dissipate in strengthFover time means that in
dicated that children with more experience in center the case of nonrelative care, it is center care that has
settings continued to manifest somewhat more problem unique a and enduring impact of a seemingly ad-
behaviors through sixth grade. The fact that this result verse kind.
was not moderated by age means that this seemingly One possible reason why relations between center
adverse consequence of center-based care did not care and problem behavior may remain is that pri-
dissipate as did so many other effects of amount of mary school teachers lack the training as well as the
child care on social functioning detected previously. time to address behavior problems, given their pri-
Because it was level of reported problems as mea- mary focus on academics (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta,
sured on a continuous scale that was the outcome to 2001). Ultimately, it will be important to identify the
be explained in this inquiry, not clinical levels of specific mechanisms that link center-care experience
problem behavior, no claim can or should be made with problem behavior. Previous analyses show that
on the basis of this report that center-based child care even though time in nonmaternal care is related to
contributes to or predicts psychopathology. somewhat less harmonious patterns of mother –
Although it is not entirely clear why the predictive child interaction in the first 3 years of life (NICHD
power of center-care experience vis-à-vis problem Early Child Care Research Network, 1998) and to
behavior remains unchanged through sixth grade, somewhat elevated rates of insecure infant – mother
the fact that it does is consistent with van IJzendoorn attachment when they coincide with low levels of
et al.’s (2004) secondary analysis of 54-months data maternal sensitivity (NICHD Early Child Care Re-
from the NICHD SECCYD. Recall that these inves- search Network, 1997), it is not via attachment or
tigators found that predictive links between overall parenting that time in child careFor in center care
time in any kind of care and problem behavior re- in particularFseems to operate when it comes to
ported by the NICHD Early Child Care Research predicting problem behavior. As we noted in an
Network (2003a, 2003b, 2003c) were accounted for earlier report, the actual mechanism of influence by
not just by time in nonrelative care in general, but which quantity of careFor in the current report,
time in center care in particular. The fact that the experience in center careFexerts the detected
findings from the secondary analysis reported herein ‘‘effect’’ remains somewhat of a mystery (NICHD
(Table 7) showed that effects of time spent in any Early Child Care Research Network, 2003b), al-
kind of nonrelative care on teacher-reported exter- though some subsequent work by the NICHD Early
nalizing problems became insignificant by sixth Child Care Research Network (2007) highlights the
grade, as did effects of nonrelative care on teacher – potential role played by agemates and, thus, by peer
child conflict, social – emotional adjustment, and processes.
academic work habits further underscores the Whatever the reasons that quality and type of
uniquely enduring effect of center-based care. In- care remained associated with children’s vocabulary
deed, when considered in their entirety, the results of and problem behavior, respectively, the modest
698 Belsky et al.

magnitude of all effect sizes involving child care that relations between child care and child devel-
cannot be ignored. Even though there are certainly opment that were present early, then disappeared by
grounds for questioning the developmental signifi- the late-elementary-school years, could re-emerge in
cance of the enduring ‘‘effects’’ detected, we regard adolescence. At the same time, it must be acknowl-
them as noteworthy and meaningful because of the edged that even the small associations that endure to
large number of children in America who experience sixth grade may themselves attenuate to the point of
extensive and/or low-quality child care before becoming nonsignificant or, if they remain, be of no
school entry. This contemporary situation raises functional significance to children’s development,
questions about the potential collective consequen- either in terms of individual functioning or the
cesFacross classrooms, schools, communities, and functioning of groups of children. Examination of
society at largeFof small enduring developmental social and academic functioning in middle school
differences among children who vary in their early and high school, therefore, represents an important
child-care experience (Belsky, 2001; NICHD SEC- next step in understanding the relations between
CYD, 2003a, 2006). What happens if early child care child care and developmental outcomes.
affects many children in small ways? Do teachers One new relation between early child care and
provide higher or lower levels of instruction, spend children’s development, representing a possible
more or less time managing their classes? Are play- ‘‘sleeper effect,’’ emerged in this inquiry. Children
grounds more or less friendly places in which to whose child-care hours increased between 3 and 54
spend time? To address these issues, future research months of age scored somewhat lower on vocabu-
will need to focus on classroom and playground lary in fifth grade. This is the first time that a link has
dynamics instead of focusing solely on individual been detected between the amount of care and aca-
child outcomes. demic functioning (or cognitive functioning more
In addition to revealing two associations between generally) in the NICHD SECCYD. This fact, coupled
child-care experience and child development that with the findings from the follow-up analyses,
endured over time, the results also indicated, as al- showing the effect in question was an apparent ar-
ready noted, that some previously detected relations tifact of other factors leads us not to offer an expla-
became weaker across the elementary school years. nation of it.
In particular, relations linking more time spent in Associations linking child-care experience with
any kind of child care to somewhat higher levels of child development in the late-elementary-school
behavior problems, which had been evident before years were smaller in size and less pervasive than
school entry and in kindergarten and first grade, those associated with families and parenting (see
attenuated, and were no longer statistically signifi- Table 6, bottom subsection). This is not surprising
cant in fifth (or even third) grade. The same was true not only because such results are consistent with
with respect to links between amount of care and earlier study findings but because children are being
teachers’ reports of conflicted relationships with the studied 6 – 7 years after leaving child care for ele-
study children and between quality of care and mentary school. Most of the study children were
reading skills. Caution seems warranted before enrolled in more than one child-care setting before
concluding definitively that these earlier detected they started school and experienced multiple differ-
associations have permanently disappeared, how- ent classrooms and after-school arrangements sub-
ever, if only because at earlier developmental periods sequent to school entry. On average, in fact, study
we found that significant relations between child children experienced more than five different care
care and child development that had seemingly arrangements between 3 and 54 months of age and
disappeared subsequently re-emerged (NICHD six different classrooms between kindergarten and
Early Child Care Research Network, 2003a). More- fifth grade. In comparison, family experiences and
over, developmental theorists have posited that im- parenting were relatively stable. Parents and chil-
portant transitions, such as beginning a new school, dren also share genes, further contributing to the
entering puberty, or dealing with adolescence, more relative strength of associations between parenting
generally can create challenges in which ‘‘old’’ issues and child functioning through sixth grade.
are resurrected (Caspi, 1998). The fact that transition Presuming, as seems likely, that links between
to middle and high school results in less teacher parenting quality and child development are not
oversight and support for academic achievement, entirely a function of shared biology, the parenting
creating challenges for youths who require more results emerging from this study of child care high-
scaffolding to maintain their academic achievement light the potential for interventions aimed at en-
(Roeser, Eccles, & Sameroff, 2000), raises the prospect hancing parenting to yield greater developmental
Early Child Care 699

benefits for children than ones geared toward mod- dergarten adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 30,
ifying child care, perhaps by improving child-care 690 – 700.
quality. It is probably misguided, however, to pit Belsky, J. (1986). Infant day care: A cause for concern? Zero
these two intervention strategies against one another, to Three, 6, 1 – 7.
especially because efforts made to enhance the Belsky, J. (1988). The ‘‘effects’’ of infant day care recon-
quality of parenting do not preclude efforts to sidered. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 3, 235 – 272.
modify the child-care experience. Moreover, syner- Belsky, J. (1990). Parental and nonparental care and chil-
dren’s socioemotional development: A decade in review.
gistic effects may emerge when both avenues of in-
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 885 – 903.
tervention are pursued simultaneously (Love et al.,
Belsky, J. (2001). Developmental risks (still) associated with
2005).
early child care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Despite the many strengths of the NICHD SEC- 42, 845 – 859.
CYDFincluding a large, diverse sample, a pro- Blau, D. (1999). The effects of child care characteristics on
spective longitudinal design, a rich array of child development. Journal of Human Resources, 34, 786 –
measures obtained from multiple methods, and 822.
multiple respondentsFit has limitations. The sam- Borge, A. I., Rutter, M., Cote, S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2004).
ple was not specifically drawn to be nationally rep- Early childcare and physical aggression: Differentiating
resentative. Also, the study design is correlational; social selection from social causation. Journal of Child
therefore, causal inferences can only be drawn with Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 367 – 376.
caution, if at all. Furthermore, and as already noted, Broberg, A. G., Wessels, H., Lamb, M. E., & Hwang, C. P.
the study only addresses relations between child (1997). Effects of day care on the development of cog-
care and child development at the level of individual nitive abilities in 8-year-olds: A longitudinal study. De-
children, not potentially cumulative effects on larger velopmental Psychology, 33, 62 – 69.
social groupings of children. Nonetheless, the re- Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2002). Hierarchical linear
peated comprehensive measurements of environ- models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.).
mental contexts of child care, school, and family in Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Riggins, R., Zeisel, S.,
conjunction with repeated assessments of both cog-
Neebe, E., & Bryant, M. (2000). Relating quality of center
nitive and social functioning have provided a unique
child care to early cognitive and language development
opportunity to address the issues of potentially en-
longitudinally. Child Development, 71, 339 – 357.
during or dissipating effects of early experience after Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home observation
taking into consideration many confounding and for measurement of the environment. Little Rock: University
competing explanatory factors. The NICHD Early of Arkansas at Little Rock.
Child Care Research Network continues to monitor Campbell, F. A., Pungello, E. P., Miller-Johnson, S.,
the study children’s development into middle ado- Burchinal, M. R., & Ramey, C. (2001). The development
lescence to determine whether experiences in early of cognitive and academic abilities: Growth curves from
child care (its quality, quantity, or type) relate to an early intervention educational experiment. Develop-
academic performance in high school and whether mental Psychology, 37, 231 – 242.
the behavior problems associated with early center Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life
care presage problem behaviors in high school. course. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.),
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and
personality development (5th ed., pp. 342 – 388). New York:
References Wiley.
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Teacher’s Report Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1989). Infant day care: Maligned or
Form and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont malignant? American Psychologist, 44, 266 – 273.
Coie, J., & Dodge, K. (1988). Multiple sources of data on
Department of Psychiatry.
social behavior and social status in the school: A cross-
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing
age comparison. Child Development, 59, 815 – 829.
and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Colwell, M., Pettit, G., Meece, D., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K.
Andersson, B. (1992). Effects of day care on cognitive and
A. (2001). Cumulative risk and continuity in nonparen-
socioemotional competence of thirteen-year-old Swed- tal care from infancy to early adolescence. Merrill-Palmer
ish schoolchildren. Child Development, 63, 2036. Quarterly, 47, 207 – 234.
Bacharach, V. R., & Baumeister, A. A. (2003). Child care and Committee on Family and Work Policies (2003). Working
severe externalizing behavior in kindergarten children. families and growing kids: Caring for children and adoles-
Applied Developmental Psychology, 23, 527 – 537. cents. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Bates, J. E., Marvinney, D., Kelly, T., Dodge, K. A., Bennett, Crockenberg, S. C. (2003). Rescuing the baby from the
D. S., & Pettit, G. S. (1994). Child-care history and kin- bathwater: How gender and temperament (may influ-
700 Belsky et al.

ence how child care affects child development. Child NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003a).
Development, 74, 1034 – 1038. Child care and mother-child interaction from 36 months
Deater-Deckard, K. A., Pinkerton, R., & Scarr, S. (1996). through first grade. Infant Behavior and Development, 26,
Child care quality and children’s behavioral adjustment: 345 – 370.
A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychol- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003b). Does
ogy and Psychiatry, 37, 937 – 948. amount of time spent in child care predict socioemo-
Egeland, B., & Hiester, M. (1995). The long-term conse- tional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten?
quences of infant day-care and mother – infant attach- Child Development, 74, 976 – 1005.
ment. Child Development, 66, 474 – 485. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2003c).
Fox, N., & Fein, G. (Eds.), (1990). Infant day care: The current Families matterFeven for kids in child care. Journal of
debate. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 24, 58 – 62.
Greenspan, S. I. (2003). Child care research: A clinical NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2004a). Does
perspective. Child Development, 74, 1064 – 1068. class size in first grade relate to children’s academic and
Gresham, FM., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rating social performance or observed classroom processes?
system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Developmental Psychology, 40, 651 – 664.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences. Bal- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004b). Type
timore: Brooks. of child care and children’s development at 54 months.
Haskins, R. (1985). Public school aggression among chil- Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 203 – 230.
dren with varying day-care experience. Child Develop- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004c). COS
ment, 56, 689 – 703. qualitative scales and reliabilities observational ratings of the
Howes, C. (1988). Relations between early child care and
fifth grade classroom. Child Care Data Report 571. Research
schooling. Developmental Psychology, 24, 53 – 57.
Triangle Park, NC: RTI International. Retrieved March
Langlois, J. H., & Liben, L. S. (2003). Child care re-
15, 2005, from https://secc.rti.org/project/display.
search: An editorial perspective. Child Development, 74,
cfm?t=c&i=ccdr571
969 – 1226.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (Ed.) (2005a).
Lazar, I., & Darlington, R. (1982). Lasting effects of early
Child care and child development: Results of the NICHD
education. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
study of early child care and youth development. New York:
Development, Serial No. 195, Vol. 47, Nos.2 – 3.
Guilford Press.
Love, J. M., Harrison, L., Sagi_Schwartz, A., van IJzendo-
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005b). Early
orn, M. H., Ross, C., Ungerer, J. A., et al. (2003). Child
child care and children’s development in the primary
care quality matters: How conclusions may vary with
context. Child Development, 74, 1021 – 1033. grades: Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child
Love, J. M., Kisker, E. E., Ross, C., Raikes, H., Constantine, Care. American Educational Research Journal, 43, 537 – 570.
J., Boller, K., et al. (2005). The effectiveness of Early Head NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005c). A
Start for 3-year-old children and their parents. Develop- day in third grade: A large-scale study of classroom
mental Psychology, 41, 885 – 901. quality and teacher and student behavior. The Elementary
Maccoby, E. E., & Lewis, C. C. (2003). Less day care or School Journal, 105, 305 – 323.
different day care? Child Development, 74, 1069 – 1075. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2006). Child
McGrew, K. S., Werder, J. K., & Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Care Effect Sizes for the NICHD Study of Early Child
WJ-R technical manual. Allen, TX: DLM. Care and Youth Development. American Psychologist, 61,
National Reading Panel – Report of the Subgroups (2000). 99 – 116.
Teaching children to read: An evidence based assessment of the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2007). Testing
scientific research literature on reading and its implications a series of causal propositions relating time spent in child care
for reading instruction (NIH Pub. No. 004754). Washing- to children’s externalizing behavior. Manuscript submitted
ton, DC: NCIHD. for publication.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1997). The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network & Duncan, G.
effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment J. (2003). Modeling the impacts of child care quality on
security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child children’s preschool cognitive development. Child De-
Care. Child Development, 68, 860 – 879. velopment, 74, 1454 – 1475.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1998). Early Peisner-Feinberg, E., & Burchinal, M. (1997). Concurrent
child care and self-control, compliance, and problem relations between child care quality and child outcomes:
behavior at twenty-four and thirty-six months. Child The study of cost, quality and outcomes in child care
Development, 69, 1145 – 1170. centers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 451 – 477.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2002). Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., Burchinal, M. R., Clifford, R. M.,
Early child care and children’s development prior to Culkin, M. L., Howes, C., Kagan, S. L., et al. (2001). The
school entry: Results from the NICHD Study of relation of preschool child-care quality to children’s
Early Child Care. American Education Research Journal, 39, cognitive and social developmental trajectories through
133 – 164. second grade. Child Development, 72, 1534 – 1553.
Early Child Care 701

Phillips, D., McCartney, K., Scarr, S., & Howes, C. (1987). through age 15. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1,
Selective review of infant day care research: A cause for 15 – 45.
concern. Zero to Three, 7, 18 – 21. Singer, J., & Willett, J. (2003). Applied longitudinal data
Pianta, R. C. (2001). Student-teacher relationship scale. analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York:
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Oxford University Press.
Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. T. (2000). School U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1999). Statistical abstract of the
as a context of early adolescents’ academic and social- United States. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Print-
emotional development: A summary of research find- ing Office.
ings. Elementary School Journal, 100, 443 – 471. Vandell, D. L., & Corasaniti, M. A. (1990). Variations in
Reynolds, A. (2000). Success in early childhood interventions: early child care: Do they predict subsequent social,
The Chicago Child-parent Centers. Lincoln: University of
emotional, and cognitive differences. Early Childhood
Nebraska.
Research Quarterly, 5, 555 – 572.
Rimm-Kaufman, S., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). An ecological
Vandell, D. L., Henderson, V. K., & Wilson, K. S. (1988). A
perspective on the transition to kindergarten: A theo-
longitudinal study of children with day-care experiences
retical framework to guide empirical research. Journal of
of varying quality. Child Development, 59, 1286 – 1292.
Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 491 – 511.
Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in van IJzendoorn, M. H., Tavecchio, L. W. C., Riksen-Wal-
surveys. New York: John Wiley and Sons. raven, J. M. A., Schipper, J. C., de Gevers Deynoot-Sch-
Schafer, J., & Graham, J. (2002). Missing data: Our aub, M., & Schaub, M. (11 July 2004). Center day care in
view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, the Netherlands. What do we know about its quality and ef-
147 – 177. fects? Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the
Schafer, J. L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. International Society for the Study of Behavioural De-
London: Capman & Hall. velopment. Ghent, Belgium.
Schweinhart, L. J., Weikart, D. P., & Larner, M. B. (1986). Winer, B. J. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design
Consequences of three preschool curriculum models (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen