Sie sind auf Seite 1von 2

Vol 456|13 November 2008

COMMENTARY
The innovative brain
‘Hot’ decision-making, involving the evaluation of reward and punishment, is essential to the
entrepreneurial process and may be possible to teach, argue Barbara Sahakian and her coauthors.
he creativity and innovation when computing decisions, as well as planning the details involved in opening of a

T shown by entrepreneurs is a
crucial source of employment
and economic growth. Although
emotional reactions to gains and
losses, which can greatly alter our
reasoning processes3. A growing
new company branch.
Risky or hot decisions, on the other hand,
involve an evaluation of reward and punish-
there is potential for considerable body of evidence from functional ment, with emotional significance attached
profit in making the decision to ‘go neuroimaging and studies of patients to both outcomes. For example, the decision
it alone’, rather than working for an with brain injuries points to the involve- between financing one of several potentially
existing company, these individuals accept ment of distinct processes in risky and risk- excellent but risky business opportunities is a
considerable risk. Many new ventures fail, with free decision-making. Referred to as ‘hot’ and hot decision; it is difficult to ignore emotions.
possible consequences for finances, reputation, ‘cold’ processes, these appear to be localized to Hot processes are dependent on the medial
self-esteem and even family stability. distinct regions of the brain’s frontal lobes4. and orbital sectors of the prefrontal cortex6,
Psychological and biomedical research has which lie above the orbit of the eyes and con-
traditionally considered risk-taking an abnor- All in the mind nect with limbic emotion-related areas such
mal expression of behaviour, as exemplified We measured cold processes using the one- as the amygdala.
by its association with substance abuse and touch Tower of London task (Fig. 1), which Hot processes were measured using the
bipolar mania1. We propose that entrepre- requires the participant to mentally rearrange Cambridge Gamble Task7 (Fig. 2). A partici-
neurs represent an example of highly adaptive an array of coloured balls. They are required pant is presented with an array of ten boxes
risk-taking behaviour, with positive functional to select the minimum number of moves to coloured either red or blue. A token is hid-
outcomes in the context of stressful economic complete the problem from a display across den randomly in one of the boxes, and the
decision-making. This ‘functional impulsiv- the foot of the screen. The problem difficulty participant must decide whether the token
ity’ may have evolutionary value as a means varies between one and six moves, with is in a red box, or a blue box. The ratio of
of seizing opportunities in a rapidly changing harder problems requiring greater plan- red-to-blue boxes is manipulated to present
environment. ning, working memory and decision- different levels of uncertainty of winning.
Here, we present preliminary neuro- making skills. Participants then choose a bet to stake
cognitive data from matched groups of Performance on this task on the colour decision. A displayed

B. MELLOR
entrepreneurs and managerial controls, reflects cold processes that points total reminds participants how
which highlight the potential are dependent upon the well they are doing. Thus, on each
of this approach to examine dorsolateral prefron- trial, subjects must balance the
advantageous risk-taking. Our tal cortex5. These are chance of gaining more points
16-member entrepreneurial emotionally neutral with a high bet against the chance
group from the ‘Silicon Fen’2 decisions with no weigh- of being incorrect and losing
cluster of high-tech companies ing of rewards and punish- their wager.
around Cambridge, UK, consists ments. Cold processes govern Our groups of entre-
of individuals who have founded real-life decisions such as preneurs and managers
at least two companies. Seven- showed comparable per-
teen non-founding formance on the Tower of
managers lacking London test of cold proc-
the experience of esses, with no differences
venture creation were in the number of solutions
recruited as a comparison solved at the first attempt.
group who were matched to On the Cambridge Gamble
the entrepreneurs for demo- Task, both groups were able to
graphic characteristics including make high-quality decisions,
age and intelligence, estimated with the selecting the majority colour
National Adult Reading Test. at least 95% of the time. The
All the participants completed a computer- remaining 5% is likely to be
ized neurocognitive assessment that measured accounted for by ‘gambler’s
various aspects of their decision-making fallacy’ in which subjects try
abilities. Classical models of decision-making to second guess the compu-
from psychology and economics focus on the ter by choosing the less likely
computation of expected values for each deci- colour. However, when sub-
sion option, based on the relative gains and jects were introduced to the
losses of the associated outcomes. But these hot components of the task, dif-
models fail to accommodate emotional states ferences were observed. We found
168
NATURE|Vol 456|13 November 2008 OPINION

that entrepreneurs behaved in a significantly a risk tolerance that we have seen in our study?
riskier way, betting a greater percentage of Are entrepreneurs conscious risk seekers,
their accrued points (63%) than their manage- or do they place less weight on risk in their
rial counterparts (51%). Both groups adjusted decision-making?
their wagering according to the likelihood of There are courses that teach the ‘know-how’
success (dependent on the ratio of red-to-blue of entrepreneurship and, within this, teach
boxes). The only performance difference was how to mitigate risk through extensive busi-
the amount that was bet. ness planning and market research. Yet given
Interestingly, this risk-taking performance this new evidence, courses teaching the oppo-
in the entrepreneurs was accompanied by site — risk-tolerance in both behaviour and
elevated scores on personality impulsiveness b personality — may also be desirable. Indeed,
measures and superior cognitive-flexibility this may be easier to teach in young adults,
performance. We conclude that entrepreneurs in whom higher risk-taking is likely and age-
and managers do equally well when asked to appropriate. We know that perception of risk
perform cold decision-making tasks, but is particularly sensitive to framing effects;
differences emerge in the context of risky or therefore, training potential entrepreneurs to
emotional decisions. reframe their decisions may encourage greater
The pattern of performance seen on the entrepreneurial activity.
gambling task in entrepreneurs reflects a Similarly, negative perception of risk, such as
behavioural index of risk-seeking or risk tol- Figure 1 | How many moves? The Tower of that involved in starting a new venture, could
erance. Greater rewards (as well as greater London task measures ‘cold’ decision making, be mitigated by social/cultural norms. One of
losses) are available for those who bet more. asking subjects to calculate the minimum the beneficial effects of entrepreneurial clusters
A large normative study of the Cambridge number of moves required to get the array in b to in regions such as Silicon Fen may be that the
Gamble Task found that the betting behav- match that in a. This is a five-move problem. increased networking and contact amongst the
iour decreases with age across the lifespan8. entrepreneurs works to create a culture that
Comparing our recent data against this nor- normalizes a more risk-tolerant type of decision-
mative sample shows that the entrepreneurs 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 making. One might argue that business skills
(mean age 51) are comparable to the young can be acquired relatively easily from multiple
adults aged 17–27, whereas the managers sources. But if one is to boost entrepreneurial
(mean age 50.5) resemble their age-appropri- TOTAL = 100 BET = 75 activity, and if risk tolerance and the ability to
ate group (Fig. 3). These cognitive processes cope with ambiguity are barriers to achieving
are intimately linked to brain neurochemistry, a cultural shift in society, then more focus is
particularly to the neurotransmitter dopamine. needed on understanding how risk and emotion
RED BLUE
Using single-dose psychostimulants to manip- are integrated into decision-making. ■
ulate dopamine levels, we have seen modula- Andrew Lawrence, Luke Clark, Jamie Nicole
tion of risky decision-making on this task9. Labuzetta and Barbara Sahakian are at the
Therefore, it might be possible to enhance Figure 2 | Place your bets. To evaluate risk-taking, MRC-Wellcome Trust Behavioural and Clinical
entrepreneurship pharmacologically. ‘hot’ processes in the Cambridge Gamble Task Neuroscience Institute, in the Departments
(part of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test of Experimental Psychology and Psychiatry,
The dark side? Automated Battery), a randomly hidden token University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK.
Entrepreneurs demonstrate creativity through is in a blue or a red box. Subjects choose a colour e-mail: lc260@cam.ac.uk.
the development of positive financial oppor- and a stake to bet. Shai Vyakarnum is at the Centre for
tunities for society as well as themselves. Entrepreneurial Learning, Judge Business School,
Although we know little about the psycho- University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1QA, UK.
Increased risk (percent bet)

65
pathological ‘dark side’ of entrepreneurship10, 1. Paulus, M. P. Science 318, 602–606 (2007).
we would argue that the effects seen with our 60
2. Myint, Y. M., Vyakarnum, S. & New, M. J. Strategic Change
gambling task reflect functional impulsivity11, 14, 165–177 (2005).
55 3. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A. R.
a subtype of impulsiveness that may enable Science 275, 1293–1295 (1997).
impulsive individuals to capitalize on envi- 50 4. Roiser, J. P., Rubinsztein, J. S. & Sahakian, B. J. Psychiatry 2,
ronmental niches. This functional impul- 43–47 (2003).
siveness of entrepreneurs combined with 5. Baker, S. C. et al. Neuropsychologia 34, 515–526 (1996).
45
6. Rogers, R. D. et al. J. Neurosci. 19, 9029–9038 (1999).
enhanced cognitive flexibility is a winning 7. Clark, L. et al. Brain 131, 1311–1322 (2008).
combination.
rs

8. Deakin, J., Aitken, M., Robbins, T. & Sahakian, B. J.


er

–2

–5

–7
–4
eu

ag

17

53
41
28
en

Our results raise important questions: J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10, 590–598 (2004).
an
pr

9. DeVito, E. E. et al. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 636–639 (2008).


tre

Age-group
Ag
ge--group da
data
ata
if these impulsive risk-taking traits can be
En

10. Hisrich, R., Langan-Fox, J. & Grant, S. Am. Psychol. 62,


beneficial, can they be taught or otherwise 575–589 (2007).
Figure 3 | Risk taking. Entrepreneurs bet a higher 11. Dickman, S. J. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 95–102 (1990).
imparted to the potential entrepreneur? What percentage of their points than managers. This
does it take to make an entrepreneur — is it measure decreases with age in a large normative Competing financial interests: declared (see online
an inherited, inbuilt characteristic, or is it sample. Managers resemble their age-appropriate article for details).
acquired, and if so, can it be acquired by any- counterparts, whereas entrepreneurs show a risk- To comment on this article and others in our
one? What specific mechanisms lead to the tolerance similar to the youngest group. innovation series, visit http://tinyurl.com/5uolx2.

169

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen