Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

RESEARCH AND PRACTICE

Meta-Synthesis of Health Behavior Change Meta-Analyses


Blair T. Johnson, PhD, Lori A. J. Scott-Sheldon, PhD, and Michael P. Carey, PhD

Change in health behavior remains essential for the prevention of premature morbidity and mortality. A recent review suggests that (1) 50% of premature deaths can be attributed to modiable risk behaviors, (2) many health problems lack curative solutions, and (3) health prevention incurs lower costs and fewer iatrogenic effects than medical solutions.1 As a consequence, public health experts have focused their efforts on health promotion to reduce negative health outcomes. Over several decades, research on change in health behavior has burgeoned, uniting many disciplines in service of promoting health and preventing disease.24 Given the vastness of the literature, continued development of health promotion interventions depends on the ability of researchers to extract insight from prior efforts, a major challenge for researchers and public health ofcials involved in the development of behavioral health programs. Public health experts share a strong interest in the efcacy of health promotion interventions. With the growing numbers of intervention studies evaluating the success of behavior change techniques and meta-analytic reviews summarizing these studies, evaluating the efcacy of behavioral interventions can be challenging. Meta-analyses often vary by targeted health domain (e.g., condom versus alcohol use), sample (e.g., adolescents versus adults), intervention setting (e.g., community versus clinic), and assessed outcomes (e.g., single versus multiple outcomes). Moreover, some meta-analyses restrict their samples to published studies whereas others include unpublished reports. Development of health interventions is likely to be challenged by the variations among meta-analytic studies. We therefore used meta-analytic procedures to examine ndings from prior meta-analytic reviews of health promotion literatures. This integration may permit researchers, practitioners, and policymakers to determine the success of behavioral interventions more broadly. By focusing on study, sample, and

Objectives. We integrated and compared meta-analytic ndings across diverse behavioral interventions to characterize how well they have achieved change in health behavior. Methods. Outcomes from 62 meta-analyses of interventions for change in health behavior were quantitatively synthesized, including 1011 primary-level investigations with 599 559 participants. Content coding suggested 6 behavioral domains: eating and physical activity, sexual behavior, addictive behaviors, stress management, female-specic screening and intervention behaviors, and behaviors involving use of health services. Results. Behavior change interventions were efcacious (mean effect sizes = 0.080.45). Behavior change was more evident in more recent metaanalyses; those that sampled older interventions and literatures or sampled more published articles; those that included studies that relied on self-report, used briefer interventions, or sampled fewer, older, or female participants; and in some domains (e.g., stress management) more than others (e.g., sexual behaviors). Conclusions. Interventions improved health-related behaviors; however, efcacy varied as a function of participant and intervention characteristics. This meta-synthesis provides information about the efcacy of behavioral change interventions across health domains and populations; this knowledge can inform the design and development of public health interventions and future meta-analyses of these studies. (Am J Public Health. 2010;100:21932198. doi:10. 2105/AJPH.2008.155200)

intervention characteristics reported in metaanalyses of health behaviors, this metasynthesis may assist researchers in the design and development of behavior change interventions, thus having the potential to improve the science of health promotion; it can serve a similar function for future meta-analyses of related literatures. In our meta-synthesis, we focus on behavioral outcomes, the most denitive gauge of the success of an intervention,5 and examine factors that moderate intervention efcacy.

METHODS
Meta-analyses were obtained through computerized databases and manual searches of journals. To qualify for inclusion, meta-analyses had to (1) include studies sampling participants only from the general population, rather than institutionalized samples (e.g., inmates);

(2) focus on primary or secondary prevention of a behavioral change domain (i.e., appear in one of the Healthy People 2010 6 overarching categories); (3) review a total of 5 or more studies measuring risk reduction behavior (e.g., exercise) or behavioral tendency (e.g., weight loss); (4) include studies evaluating interventions compared with a comparison or control group (i.e., between-group studies only); (5) review psychological, educational, or behavioral interventions; and (6) provide sufcient statistical information to estimate a weighted mean effect size (d+).7 When meta-analyses lacked sufcient study details or effect sizes, meta-analysts were contacted for additional information. Meta-analyses that (1) used biomedical interventions exclusively,8 (2) focused on mental health outcomes (e.g., depression9), or (3) evaluated mass media campaigns10 were excluded. By the close of our search, 65 meta-analyses met our inclusion criteria; however, as 3 did not t the

November 2010, Vol 100, No. 11 | American Journal of Public Health

Johnson et al. | Peer Reviewed | Research and Practice | 2193

RESEARCH AND PRACTICE

6 targeted categories of health behavior change,1113 the nal sample comprised 62 metaanalyses. (For details, see Technical Summary, available as a supplement to the online version of this article at http://www.ajph.org.)

Coding and Reliability


Two independent raters coded each metaanalysis along several dimensions (j = 0.90). First, the behavioral change domain comprised 6 categories, including female-specic behaviors (e.g., mammography), addictions, eating and physical activity, sexual behavior, stress management, and improving health services or patients participation in them. To the extent that each study within the meta-analyses permitted it, we also coded mean values of (1) study characteristics (published study, randomized controlled trial, self-report [versus objective measures], and number of authors); (2) sample characteristics (sample size, proportion women, and age in years); and (3) intervention characteristics (number of sites, brief interventionsthat is, proportion with 1 hour or less of content). Numbers of sites and authors are aspects of organizational span. We also coded the date of the earliest study included in each meta-analysis. We examined linear and logarithmic trends for sample size for the studies within each meta-analysis.14 To gauge the age of the literature, the date of the rst study was subtracted from the publication date of the meta-analysis.

Study Treatment and Analyses


To equate the comparisons among metaanalyses, we took the following actions. Most meta-analyses provided a single behavioral index for each study; if more than 1 was provided and these were highly correlated (e.g., pregnancy rates, sexual activity),15 we averaged these effects. Some syntheses analyzed intervention effects across time by separating their studies results into immediate and delayed measures16; in this study, we included only the rst follow-up assessment. We used betweengroup mean effect sizes when possible, because they represent a more conservative estimate of intervention efcacy and because they were most often reported.17 Some meta-analyses included effect sizes from multiple measures of the same outcome (e.g., frequency and last-time condom use) without averaging them,18 thereby

violating the meta-analytic assumption of nonindependence.19 In these cases, we recalculated relevant statistics from the meta-analyses tables, averaging effect sizes from the same participants on the same dependent measures. Finally, when a meta-analysis reported the outcomes of more than 1 type of intervention, we kept the outcomes separate, which resulted in 7 metaanalyses with fewer than 5 studies. To calculate effect sizes, we used the standardized mean effect size (d+). Positive effect sizes indicate health improvement (e.g., weight loss) among intervention participants whereas negative effect sizes indicate health impairment (e.g., weight gain) compared with controls. We used meta-analytic statistics with conventional weights calculated to reect the sample sizes of each meta-analysis (i.e., each mean effect sizes inverse variance).7 Our main analyses followed xed-effects models to examine mean tendencies and moderators of mean effect sizes; these models assume that variation appears only from sampling error.20,21 For each metaanalysis, we report I 2, which gauges whether the mean effect size ts its underlying effect sizes well22; inferences from I 2 are equivalent to the Q statistic,23 but values no longer depend on the number of studies included in the meta-analysis (k). I 2 provides information about homogeneity on a 0 to 100 scale; high values signify more variation in study outcomes than expected by sampling error alone and are interpreted as heterogeneity (see Technical Summary). Analyses based on coded dimensions of the meta-analyses relied on the observed values for each dimension, considering each dimension as the sole predictor of d+, with 2 exceptions: (1) sample size, for which both linear and log-linear components appeared, and (2) age, for which linear, quadratic, and cubic terms appeared. Estimation of a quadratic trend requires the linear trend be simultaneously modeled, and estimation of a cubic trend requires all 3 trends be modeled simultaneously; these values were zero-centered prior to calculation of the quadratic and cubic terms.

outcomes such as smoking, exercise, diet, weight loss, stress, participation in mammography screening or other medical intervention (e.g., vaccination), sexual behavior, alcohol use, and drug abuse (see Table A1, available as a supplement to the online version of this article at http://www.ajph.org). Meta-analyses of individual health behavior domains included as few as 2 and as many as 120 studies (median =12) and typically sampled studies published in journals (89%). In total, 599 559 participants were included in the meta-analyses. Average sample size was 9670 participants per meta-analysis, with individual studies sampling as few as 31 or as many as 4427 participants (median = 281). Most interventions were brief (86%), were coauthored (mean number of authors = 4.57, range =1.7926.17), investigated using randomized controlled trials (76%), and were conducted at more than 1 site (83%). Study outcomes were predominately assessed by self-report (80%). Fifty-three percent of participants were female and 47% were male; most (74%) were adults (mean age = 37 years; SD =15.81; range =1059). Participants in sexual behavior studies were younger (mean = 21.53 years) than those in studies examining other behaviors (mean = 46.13 years; F1,40 = 56.48; P < .001; d = 2.34).

Overall Efcacy Across Meta-Analyses


Across the 62 meta-analyses and their 1011 studies, intervention participants signicantly adopted healthier behaviors (d+ = 0.21; P < .001); the amount of change varied across the meta-analyses (I 2 = 93%; 95% condence interval [CI] = 92.4, 93.0). No meta-analysis had signicantly negative outcomes, and most (89%; k = 51) conrmed statistically signicant health promotion effects (Table A1, available as a supplement to the online version of this article at http://www.ajph.org). Nineteen (31%) of the 62 meta-analyses had nonsignicant homogeneity (I 2) statistics; for each of these domains, the mean d+ reasonably characterized its effects. For the remaining 46 meta-analyses, outcomes varied more widely around the mean d+ than would be expected by sampling error alone, with some indicating more and others indicating less efcacy. The number of studies in a meta-analysis did not correlate with I 2 (r = 0.10), but meta-analyses with signicant I 2 values had larger samples of studies.

RESULTS
Meta-analyses evaluated cognitive or behavioral interventions, health or resistance education, motivational enhancement, relapse prevention, or skills training, and examined

2194 | Research and Practice | Peer Reviewed | Johnson et al.

American Journal of Public Health | November 2010, Vol 100, No. 11

RESEARCH AND PRACTICE

Factors Linked With Intervention Efcacy


A series of moderator models established the extent to which variation in the magnitude of mean effect sizes related to methodological features. Health behavior domain. Efcacy depended markedly on health domain (Table 1). The largest effects appeared for meta-analyses addressing stress management (mean d+ = 0.45). Meta-analyses aimed at improving health services or patients participation in them produced a smaller effect (d+ = 0.35). Health promotion meta-analyses addressing behaviors for women, addictions, and eating and physical activity had a d+ of about 0.20, whereas meta-analyses of sexual behavior interventions yielded the smallest (but signicantly positive) effects (d+ = 0.08). Study features. Meta-analyses that included more published studies had larger effect sizes than those with fewer published studies (Table 2). Objective measures of health behavior (e.g., weight change) tended to have smaller effect sizes than meta-analyses relying on self-reports (e.g., physical activity). Meta-analyses that included more studies with randomized controlled trial designs achieved larger mean effect sizes than those that included fewer randomized controlled trials. Finally, meta-analyses of more established literatures obtained larger effects than those reviewing newly emerging interventions. Meta-analyses published more recently documented larger effects, as did those with more studies evaluating adults and females. Meta-analyses of studies with briefer interventions recorded larger effects; indeed, those sampling only studies with long interventions exhibited an effect size that did not differ from zero. Similarly, meta-analyses were more efcacious when they included studies with fewer research sites; smaller effects were found for studies with multiple sites. Meta-analyses based on studies with more authors documented larger effects than those with fewer authors. Finally, meta-analyses sampling studies with fewer participants recorded larger effects, a negatively accelerated pattern that persisted when the linear effect of sample size was controlled. Although meta-analyses sampling more published studies yielded larger mean effect sizes, simultaneously controlling for this

TABLE 1Health Behavior Change Efcacy by Health Behavior Domain in 62 Meta-Analyses


Targeted Health Behavior Domain Stress management Improving participation in health services Eating and physical activity Addictions Screening and treatment behaviors for women Sexual behaviors No. of Meta-Analyses 5 5 12 13 9 18 d+i (95% CI) 0.45 (0.41, 0.48) 0.35 (0.34, 0.36) 0.22 (0.20, 0.23) 0.21 (0.20, 0.22) 0.21 (0.20, 0.23) 0.08 (0.07, 0.09) QBa 16.97 405.05 97.11 1764.04 126.34 187.89

Note. CI = condence interval; d+i = weighted mean effect size (positive for improvements in the outcome studied and negative for impairments). Health behavior domains are ranked from highest to lowest efcacy. For health behavior domain, R2 = 0.36 (P < .001), QResidual = 2597.39 (P < .001). R2 is the amount of variance explained by the meta-analytic features; QResidual is the model-t statistic. Signicance of R2 implies the presence of moderation; signicance of QResidual implies incorrect model t, such that there is more variation remaining than expected by sampling error alone. a The between-groups-of-studies index, QB, indicates the extent to which the mean effect sizes within the behavior change target area vary (P < .001).

effect left the log-linear pattern intact (P < .001). Similarly, the negatively accelerated curve was still present in meta-analyses based entirely on journal-published studies, suggesting that it does not purely reect publication or other reporting bias.

DISCUSSION
Behavior change remains pivotal to disease prevention. Growing numbers of primary-level interventions and meta-analyses of those trials allowed us to gauge the state of the science of health behavior change. Our analyses yielded 3 clear ndings. First, behavioral interventions promote healthy behaviors and reduce risky behavior, with effects ranging in size from small to medium. Second, the efcacy of health behavior change varies across the following behavioral domains: eating and physical activity, sexual behavior, addictive behaviors, stress management, female-specic health behaviors, and health service use. Third, differences in target-specic effect sizes can be explained by the characteristics of the research literatures, including longer history of investigation, more published articles, use of self-report, sample composition (larger, older, more females), and intervention duration; efcacy was also higher in more recent meta-analyses.

Meta-analyses of stress management interventions showed they were more successful than those including interventions for other behaviors. Because stress management often involves thoughts and emotions, change in this domain may be easier to achieve relative to change in behavior. Changing behavior is typically more difcult than is changing attitudes or stress appraisals, especially when the behavior becomes habitual.24 Further, changing sexual behavior, a domain for which the lowest overall efcacy was achieved, may be the most challenging, partly because it involves another person. However, more recent sexualrisk reduction interventions2527 have documented a larger impact than the current study showed, suggesting greater impact can be achieved with sexual behaviors.

Participant Characteristics
Age related to the efcacy of behavior change, with efcacy increasing from childhood and adolescence through late middle age (range =1059 years). The analysis is cross-sectional rather than longitudinal, but observed trends corroborate trends observed by developmental psychologists. 28 Younger people may take more risks because of the developmental process (e.g., exploring relationships) and because they have less experience and thus fewer negative experiences than older adults, who have learned to avoid unnecessary risks; developmental experts recognize that, as experience with health risks

Targeted Health Behavior Domain


Differences in health promotion efcacy varied by targeted behavior (Table 1).

November 2010, Vol 100, No. 11 | American Journal of Public Health

Johnson et al. | Peer Reviewed | Research and Practice | 2195

RESEARCH AND PRACTICE

Intervention Length TABLE 2Health Behavior Change Efcacy by Basic and Sample Features in 62 Meta-Analyses
Features of Meta-Analyses Published in journals, % (n = 62) 100 45 Studies using self-report outcome measures, % (n = 52) 0 100 Studies using RCT designs, % (n = 54) 16.7 100 Age of literature, y (n = 62) 4 28 Year of meta-analysis (n = 62) 1988 2003 Mean age of sample, y (n = 42)c 10 18 36 59 Mean % of sample female (n = 43) All female (0% male) All male (0% female) 0.02 (0.01, 0.05) 0.09 (0.07, 0.10) 0.14 (0.13, 0.16) 0.27 (0.26, 0.29) 0.15 0.16 (0.15, 0.17) 0.09 (0.07, 0.12) 0.01 (0.00, 0.02) 0.45 (0.43, 0.46) 0.22 0.14 (0.13, 0.15) 0.26 (0.25, 0.27) 0.67c 0.16 (0.14, 0.18) 0.27 (0.26, 0.27) 0.60 0.23 (0.22, 0.24) 0.17 (0.17, 0.18) 0.15 0.26 (0.25, 0.27) 0.01 (0.01, 0.02) 0.16 d+i (95% CI)a bb 0.44

Contrary to expectations, meta-analyses including briefer interventions had greater efcacy than those sampling longer interventions, even when other study features were controlled for (see Table A2, available as a supplement to the online version of this article at http://www.ajph.org). Intervention dose varied widely, but most meta-analyses included studies with less than 1 hour of intervention time. It is possible that intervention length is an artifact of assessment interval; that is, briefer interventions tended to have shorter follow-up periods. Because initial behavior change is easier to achieve and maintenance is more challenging, shorter interventions may appear more efcacious because of briefer follow-up intervals.30 We could not examine this possibility because duration between the intervention and the measures was not commonly reported. Thus, our ndings on the stability of behavior change are less informative than optimal.

Sample Size
That most behavioral interventions are successful suggests that larger-scale interventions based on these models would also prove successful if implemented with delity. However, our nding that efcacy decreases as study sample size increases suggests that the impact of health promotion interventions is attenuated in larger group contexts.31 Metaanalyses based on interventions focusing on individuals or small groups may succeed better because participants are more likely to attend to (and benet from) the intervention, and because facilitators can tailor content to the participant. Thus, small group sizes may inuence participants motivation and encourage greater participation, a pattern that has been observed in other contexts32,33 and is consistent with research on client characteristics affecting psychotherapy outcomes.34 Most meta-analyses in our sample did not report on studies tailoring their interventions, making this hypothesis difcult to test. Alternatively, to ensure treatment delity in larger groups, researchers may forgo tailoring intervention content to the target group, potentially decreasing the interventions efcacy.35 Regardless of the interpretation of our group size effect, our results suggest that when behavioral interventions are disseminated, the

Note. CI = condence interval; RCT = randomized controlled trial; d+i = weighted mean effect size (positive for improvements in the outcome studied and negative for impairments); n = number of meta-analyses. Each model considers each meta-analysis feature independently rather than simultaneous with the other meta-analysis features. a Displayed values are observations at representative low and high extremes of the study dimension in question, where the dimension is represented in its continuous form. b All values in this column reach statistical signicance (P < .001) and are standardized regression coefcients, with the 1 footnoted exception. c Based on the cubic regression equation, which also includes the linear and quadratic terms; for this model, b represents the multiple R of the 3 terms.

accumulate across the life span, perceptions of vulnerability increase and healthy behaviors are adopted to minimize aging-related changes.29 The inclusion of HIV and pregnancy prevention interventions helped to create this age-related pattern because participants targeted by such interventions tend to be younger. An investigation of this pattern within the sexual behavior meta-analyses suggests that it matches the overall age ndings. Overall, meta-analyses suggest that interventions succeed better when implemented with female participants, a pattern maintained when other aspects of the meta-analyses are statistically controlled. Because

some interventions were implemented only with women (e.g., mammography screening), we examined whether gender inuenced outcomes when women-only interventions were omitted. Surprisingly, the relation between gender and efcacy reversed (b = 0.40; P < .001). It is unclear why the pattern would change across the other health behavior domains (Table 1), but explanations include the possibilities that these other domains may be more relevant to men or that the interventions lacked sensitivity to key aspects of gender. Continued research on gender differences in response to health promotion interventions is encouraged.

2196 | Research and Practice | Peer Reviewed | Johnson et al.

American Journal of Public Health | November 2010, Vol 100, No. 11

RESEARCH AND PRACTICE

largest effects may be seen when interventions are implemented in small-group and individualized circumstances such as individual counseling.

Other Factors Underlying Efcacy


Several other factors inuenced the efcacy of the interventions we reviewed. First, as found in other studies,36 meta-analyses based on published studies achieved larger effects than those with fewer journal reports. This pattern may reect a reporting bias, or it may be that unpublished reports are often marked by lower methodological quality. Meta-analyses based more on self-report measures tended to achieve larger effects than other outcomes, a pattern that may reect social demand or positive self-appraisals to look better to others or oneself.37 One might expect that such motives are strongest in younger people, who need social acceptance. Indeed, reliance on self-reports had a positive relation to efcacy for metaanalyses with participants aged younger than 30 years (b = 0.43; P < .001) whereas it had a slight negative relation in those with older participants (b = 0.06; P < .01). Studies characterized by larger organizational spans (e.g., more research sites) reported reduced efcacy. Overall, this relation was observed, but it disappeared when other dimensions were controlled. Another way to gauge organizational span is by counting the number of authors associated with study reports. Indeed, number of authors positively related to the magnitude of effect sizes, consonant with organizational research.38 We also found larger effect sizes in meta-analyses that depended on randomized controlled trials, but this effect disappeared when other features of the studies were controlled (see Table A3, available as a supplement to the online version of this article at http://www.ajph.org).39 We found that meta-analyses based on older literatures had larger effects than more recent meta-analyses. Together, these ndings offer optimism that newer research domains with modest behavior change improve over time as interventionists rene their techniques, as suggested by the recent sexual-risk-reduction metaanalyses highlighted in the Results section. Earlier studies may also reect more self-initiated change, easier change, or change from

less well-established behaviors.40 For example, in the area of smoking cessation, it is recognized that cessation trials today probably include more dependent smokers than early quitters.41,42 Finally, recent studies may have lower efcacy because they use better control groups whose participants also exhibit positive change; type of comparison group could not be evaluated because meta-analyses seldom focused on the control group. Future meta-analyses should pay greater attention to the recommendations emphasized by research checklists (e.g., CONSORT,43 TREND,44 QUOROM45); commonly examining these dimensions in meta-analytic models of the studies effects will likely improve understanding of health behavior change.

Limitations and Conclusions


Our relatively dated sample of meta-analyses may appear to be a limitation, especially giventhe growing number of meta-analyses focused on reduction of sexual risk behaviors.27 We did not update our literature search after 2003 because (1) meta-synthesis is complex and time intensive, necessitating the retrieval of information (e.g., study-level effect sizes) not routinely reported, and (2) meta-analyses on health promotion topics are becoming more complex and their numbers are increasing at an average annual rate of 17%, prohibiting a timely cumulative systematic review of the literatures.46 To address this limitation, we conducted an examination of temporal trends in the current database, which revealed that the inclusion of publication date did not alter our ndings (see Table A2, available as a supplement to the online version of this article at http://www.ajph.org). Meta-analyses have found larger effect sizes (by a rate of d+ = 0.0073 per year), yielding the prediction that metaanalyses published in 2008 ought to have an overall d+ of 0.298 (95% CI =0.286, 0.311) compared with those published in 2003 (d+ = 0.262; 95% CI =0.254, 0.270). Moreover, it is unclear why meta-analyses published after 2003 would be systematically different from those published earlier. Consequently, the current results seem unlikely to change if further metaanalyses are included; future research should test this conclusion. A related potential limitation, increasing as more meta-analyses appear, is overlapping samples of studies. An advantage of the current meta-synthesis is that samples were largely

independent. Only 8 (13%) of the meta-analyses had any studies overlapping with other meta-analyses. Statistical dependencies among samples have no bearing on whether a given meta-analytic mean was signicant, but they may affect different moderation results. Future syntheses of meta-analyses of health promotion domains will need to take great care either to examine whether dependencies between samples affect the patterns that emerge or to maintain in their samples only the most recent meta-analyses, which presumably will have larger samples. Finally, behavior change intervention outcomes are quite variable, especially for larger rather than smaller meta-analyses, which may have low power to reject the null hypothesis of homogeneity (see Technical Summary, available as a supplement to the online version of this article at http://www.ajph.org).23 Across the meta-analyses reviewed, most mean effect sizes represent general trends rather than precise point estimates. Random-effects models of these means produced values similar to the xedeffects models; therefore, this limitation is unlikely to alter the pattern of the results observed in this review. Using an unusually large data set, this investigation documents that, on the basis of the studies immediate assessments, behavioral interventions reduce health-damaging behaviors and facilitate health-promoting behaviors. Our analysis provides evidence that scientically based interventions alter behaviors that confer risk for a variety of disease endpoints. These data also show that intervention success varied across literatures, but the inconsistencies could be partially explained by dimensions that we specied in advance of our investigation. We anticipate that the ndings generated by this meta-synthesis will stimulate inquiry into the challenges and opportunities associated with health promotion, and help improve behavior change interventions. In addition, continued renement of the methods involved in syntheses of meta-analyses can increase the scientic yield of primary-level and meta-analytic research. j

About the Authors


Blair T. Johnson is with the Center for Health, Intervention, and Prevention, University of Connecticut, Storrs. Lori A. J.

November 2010, Vol 100, No. 11 | American Journal of Public Health

Johnson et al. | Peer Reviewed | Research and Practice | 2197

RESEARCH AND PRACTICE

Scott-Sheldon and Michael P. Carey are with the Center for Health and Behavior, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY. Correspondence can be sent to Blair T. Johnson, PhD, Center for Health and Behavior, University of Connecticut, 2006 Hillside Road, Unit 1248, Storrs, CT 06269-1248 (e-mail: blair.t.johnson@uconn.edu). Reprints can be ordered at http://www.ajph.org by clicking the Reprints/ Eprints link. This article was accepted August 4, 2009.

10. Snyder M. A Meta-Analysis of Media-Based HIV Prevention Education for Adolescents. Denton, TX: Texas Womans University; 1996. 11. Davis MK, Gidycz CA. Child sexual abuse prevention programs: a meta-analysis. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000; 29(2):257265. 12. Mullen PD, Simons-Morton DG, Ramirez G, Frankowski RF, Green LW, Mains DA. A meta-analysis of trials evaluating patient education and counseling for three groups of preventive health behaviors. Patient Educ Couns. 1997;32(3):157173. 13. Mytton JA, DiGuiseppi C, Gough DA, Taylor RS, Logan S. School-based violence prevention programs: systematic review of secondary prevention trials. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(8):752762. 14. Latane B. The psychology of social impact. Am Psychol. 1981;36(4):343356. 15. Franklin C, Grant D, Corcoran J, Miller POD, Bultman L. Effectiveness of prevention programs for adolescent pregnancy: a meta-analysis. J Marriage Fam. 1997;59(3):551567. 16. Black DR, Gleser LJ, Kooyers KJ. A meta-analytic evaluation of couples weight-loss programs. Health Psychol. 1990;9(3):330347. 17. Mullen PD, Mains DA, Velez R. A meta-analysis of controlled trials of cardiac patient education. Patient Educ Couns. 1992;19(2):143162. 18. Bruvold WH, Rundall TG. A meta-analysis and theoretical review of school based tobacco and alcohol intervention programs. Psychol Health. 1988;2(1):5378. 19. Becker BJ, Tinsley HEA, Brown SD. Multivariate Meta-Analysis. Handbook of Applied Multivariate Statistics and Mathematical Modeling. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2000:499525. 20. Cooper H, Hedges LV. The Handbook of Research Synthesis. New York, NY: Sage; 1994. 21. Johnson BT, Eagly AH. Quantitative synthesis of social psychological research. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, ed. Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2000:496528. 22. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21(11):15391558. 23. Huedo-Medina TB, Sanchez-Meca J, Marin-Martinez F, Botella J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol Methods. 2006;11(2): 193206. 24. Ouellette J, Wood W. Habit and intention in everyday life: the multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychol Bull. 1998;124(1):5474. 25. Johnson BT, Carey MP, Chaudoir SR, Reid AE. Sexual risk reduction for persons living with HIV: research synthesis of randomized controlled trials, 1993 to 2004. J Acquir Immune Dec Syndr. 2006;41(5):642650. 26. Johnson BT, Scott-Sheldon LA, Smoak ND, Lacroix JM, Anderson JR, Carey MP. Behavioral interventions for African Americans to reduce sexual risk of HIV: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Acquir Immune Dec Syndr. 2009;51(4):492501. 27. Noar SM. Behavioral interventions to reduce HIVrelated sexual risk behavior: review and synthesis of metaanalytic evidence. AIDS Behav. 2008;12(3):335353. 28. Reyna VF, Farley F. Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: implications for theory, practice,

and public policy. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2006;7(1): 144. 29. Olshansky SJ, Carnes BA. The Quest for Immortality: Science at the Frontiers of Aging. New York, NY: Norton; 2001. 30. Marcus BH, Dubbert PM, Forsyth LH, et al. Physical activity behavior change: issues in adoption and maintenance. Health Psychol. 2000;19(1 suppl):3241. 31. Johnson BT, Carey MP, Muellerleile PA. Large trials vs meta-analysis of smaller trials. JAMA. 1997;277(5): 277. 32. Glass GV, Smith ML. Meta-analysis of research on class size and achievement. Educ Eval Policy Anal. 1979;1:231. 33. Weisburd D, Petrosino A, Mason G. Design sensitivity in criminal justice experiments. In: Tony M, ed. Crime and Justice: A Review of Research. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago; 1993:337379. 34. Gareld SL. Research on client variables in psychotherapy. In: Bergin AE, Gareld SL, ed. Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change. 4th ed. Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons; 1994:190228. 35. Kreuter MW, Wray RJ. Tailored and targeted health communication: strategies for enhancing information relevance. Am J Health Behav. 2003;27(suppl 3):S227 S232. 36. Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. The efcacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment. Conrmation from meta-analysis. Am Psychol. 1993;48(12):11811209. 37. Schroder KE, Carey MP, Vanable PA. Methodological challenges in research on sexual risk behavior, II: accuracy of self-reports. Ann Behav Med. 2003;26(2): 104123. 38. Acedo FJ, Barroso C, Casanueva Cb, Galan JL. Coauthorship in management and organizational studies: an empirical and network analysis. J Manage Stud. 2006; 43(5):957983. 39. Heinsman DT, Shadish WR. Assignment methods in experimentation: when do nonrandomized experiments approximate answers from randomized experiments? Psychol Methods. 1996;1(2):154169. 40. Schachter S. Recidivism and self-cure of smoking and obesity. Am Psychol. 1982;37(4):436444. 41. Irvin JE, Brandon TH. The increasing recalcitrance of smokers in clinical trials. Nicotine Tob Res. 2000; 2(1):7984. 42. Irvin JE, Hendricks PS, Brandon TH. The increasing recalcitrance of smokers in clinical trials, II: pharmacotherapy trials. Nicotine Tob Res. 2003;5(1):2735. 43. Begg C, Cho M, Eastwood S, et al. Improving the quality of reporting of randomized controlled trials. The CONSORT statement. JAMA. 1996;276(8):637639. 44. Des Jarlais DC, Lyles C, Crepaz N. Improving the reporting quality of nonrandomized evaluations of behavioral and public health interventions: the TREND statement. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(3):361366. 45. Moher D, Cook DJ, Eastwood S, Olkin I, Rennie D, Stroup DF. Improving the quality of reports of metaanalyses of randomised controlled trials: the QUOROM statement. Quality of reporting of meta-analyses. Lancet. 1999;354(9193):18961900. 46. Lee WL, Bausell RB, Berman BM. The growth of health-related meta-analyses published from 1980 to 2000. Eval Health Prof. 2001;24(3):327335.

Contributors
B. T. Johnson conceptualized the study, analyzed the data, and led the writing of the article. L. A. J. Scott-Sheldon assisted with the acquisition, content coding, analysis, and interpretation of the data. M. P. Carey assisted with the conceptualization of the study and interpreted the data. All authors provided critical revisions of the article.

Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Institutes of Health grants R01-MH58563 to B. T. Johnson and K02MH01582 to M. P. Carey and facilitated by Centers for Disease Control and Prevention grant P01-CD000237 to Leslie B. Snyder. We thank the meta-analysts who made their data available for this study; Jessica M. LaCroix and Jennifer Ortiz, who assisted in coding the metaanalyses; and Marcella H. Boynton, Grant Farmer, Tania B. Huedo-Medina, Fritz Ifert-Miller, David B. Portnoy, Judy H. Tan, and Karin Weis, who provided helpful comments on a prior draft of this article.

Human Participant Protection


No protocol approval was needed for this study.

References
1. Mokdad AH, Marks JS, Stroup DF, Gerberding JL. Actual causes of death in the United States, 2000. JAMA. 2004;291(10):12381245. 2. Breslow L. The future of public health: prospects in the United States for the 1990s. Annu Rev Public Health. 1990;11:128. 3. Green LW, Kreuter MW. Health promotion as a public health strategy for the 1990s. Annu Rev Public Health. 1990;11:319334. 4. Salovey P, Rothman AJ, Rodin J. Health behavior. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, ed. The Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol 2. 4th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 1998:633683. 5. Kaplan RM. The Ziggy theorem: toward an outcomes-focused health psychology. Health Psychol. 1994; 13(6):451460. 6. US Dept of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2010: National Health Promotion and Disease Prevention Objectives. Boston, MA: Jones and Bartlett; 2000. 7. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for MetaAnalysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985. 8. Steinhart AH, Hemphill D, Greenberg GR. Sulfasalazine and mesalazine for the maintenance therapy of Crohns disease: a meta-analysis. Am J Gastroenterol. 1994;89(12):21162124. 9. North TC, McCullagh P, Tran ZV. Effect of exercise on depression. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 1990;18:379415.

2198 | Research and Practice | Peer Reviewed | Johnson et al.

American Journal of Public Health | November 2010, Vol 100, No. 11

Copyright of American Journal of Public Health is the property of American Public Health Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen