Sie sind auf Seite 1von 81

DESIGN OF EXPERIMENTS

Role of DOE in Process Improvement


DOE is a formal mathematical method for
systematically planning and conducting scientific
studies that change experimental variables
together in order to determine their effect of a
given response.
DOE makes controlled changes to input
variables in order to gain maximum amounts of
information on cause and effect relationships
with a minimum sample size.
2

Role of DOE in Process Improvement


DOE is more efficient that a standard
approach of changing one variable at a
time in order to observe the variables
impact on a given response.
DOE generates information on the effect
various factors have on a response variable
and in some cases may be able to determine
optimal settings for those factors.
3

Role of DOE in Process Improvement


DOE encourages brainstorming activities
associated with discussing key factors that may
affect a given response and allows the
experimenter to identify the key factors for
future studies.
DOE is readily supported by numerous statistical
software packages available on the market.
4

BASIC STEPS IN DOE

Four elements associated with DOE:


1. The design of the experiment,
2. The collection of the data,
3. The statistical analysis of the data, and
4. The conclusions reached and
recommendations made as a result of the
experiment.
5

TERMINOLOGY
Replication repetition of a basic
experiment without changing any factor
settings, allows the experimenter to estimate
the experimental error (noise) in the system
used to determine whether observed
differences in the data are real or just
noise, allows the experimenter to obtain
more statistical power (ability to identify
small effects)
6

TERMINOLOGY
.Randomization a statistical tool used to minimize
potential uncontrollable biases in the experiment by
randomly assigning material, people, order that
experimental trials are conducted, or any other
factor not under the control of the experimenter.
Results in averaging out the effects of the
extraneous factors that may be present in order to
minimize the risk of these factors affecting the
experimental results.
7

TERMINOLOGY
Blocking technique used to increase the
precision of an experiment by breaking the
experiment into homogeneous segments
(blocks) in order to control any potential
block to block variability (multiple lots of
raw material, several shifts, several
machines, several inspectors). Any effects
on the experimental results as a result of the
blocking factor will be identified and
minimized.
8

TERMINOLOGY
Confounding - A concept that basically means that
multiple effects are tied together into one parent
effect and cannot be separated. For example,
1. Two people flipping two different coins would
result in the effect of the person and the effect of
the coin to be confounded
2. As experiments get large, higher order
interactions (discussed later) are confounded with
lower order interactions or main effect.

TERMINOLOGY
Factors experimental factors or
independent variables (continuous or
discrete) an investigator manipulates to
capture any changes in the output of the
process. Other factors of concern are those
that are uncontrollable and those which are
controllable but held constant during the
experimental runs.

10

TERMINOLOGY
Responses dependent variable measured
to describe the output of the process.
Treatment Combinations (run)
experimental trial where all factors are set
at a specified level.

11

TERMINOLOGY

Fixed Effects Model - If the treatment


levels are specifically chosen by the
experimenter, then conclusions reached
will only apply to those levels.
Random Effects Model If the treatment
levels are randomly chosen from a
population of many possible treatment
levels, then conclusions reached can be
extended to all treatment levels in the
population.
12

PLANNING A DOE
Everyone involved in the experiment should
have a clear idea in advance of exactly what
is to be studied, the objectives of the
experiment, the questions one hopes to
answer and the results anticipated

13

PLANNING A DOE
Select a response/dependent variable
(variables) that will provide information
about the problem under study and the
proposed measurement method for this
response variable, including an
understanding of the measurement system
variability

14

PLANNING A DOE
Select the independent variables/factors
(quantitative or qualitative) to be
investigated in the experiment, the number
of levels for each factor, and the levels of
each factor chosen either specifically (fixed
effects model) or randomly (random effects
model).

15

PLANNING A DOE
Choose an appropriate experimental design
(relatively simple design and analysis methods are
almost always best) that will allow your experimental
questions to be answered once the data is collected
and analyzed, keeping in mind tradeoffs between
statistical power and economic efficiency. At this
point in time it is generally useful to simulate the
study by generating and analyzing artificial data to
insure that experimental questions can be answered
as a result of conducting your experiment
16

PLANNING A DOE
Perform the experiment (collect data)
paying particular attention such things as
randomization and measurement system
accuracy, while maintaining as uniform an
experimental environment as possible.
How the data are to be collected is a critical
stage in DOE

17

PLANNING A DOE
Analyze the data using the appropriate
statistical model insuring that attention is
paid to checking the model accuracy by
validating underlying assumptions
associated with the model. Be liberal in the
utilization of all tools, including graphical
techniques, available in the statistical
software package to insure that a maximum
amount of information is generated
18

PLANNING A DOE
Based on the results of the analysis, draw
conclusions/inferences about the results,
interpret the physical meaning of these
results, determine the practical significance
of the findings, and make recommendations
for a course of action including further
experiments

19

SIMPLE COMPARATIVE EXPERIMENTS


Single Mean Hypothesis Test
Difference in Means Hypothesis Test with
Equal Variances
Difference in Means Hypothesis Test with
Unequal Variances
Difference in Variances Hypothesis Test
Paired Difference in Mean Hypothesis Test
One Way Analysis of Variance
20

CRITICAL ISSUES ASSOCIATED WITH


SIMPLE COMPARATIVE EXPERIMENTS
How Large a Sample Should We Take?
Why Does the Sample Size Matter
Anyway?
What Kind of Protection Do We Have
Associated with Rejecting Good Stuff?
What Kind of Protection Do We Have
Associated with Accepting Bad Stuff?
21

Single Mean Hypothesis Test


After a production run of 12 oz. bottles,
concern is expressed about the possibility that
the average fill is too low.
Ho: = 12
Ha: <> 12
level of significance = = .05
sample size = 9
SPEC FOR THE MEAN: 12 + .1
22

Single Mean Hypothesis Test

Sample mean = 11.9


Sample standard deviation = 0.15
Sample size = 9
Computed t statistic = -2.0
P-Value = 0.0805162
CONCLUSION: Since P-Value > .05, you
fail to reject hypothesis and ship product.

23

Single Mean Hypothesis Test Power Curve


Power Curve
alpha = 0.05, sigma = 0.15

Power

0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
11.8

11.9

12

12.1

12.2

True Mean

24

Single Mean Hypothesis Test Power


Curve - Different Sample Sizes

25

DIFFERENCE IN MEANS - EQUAL


VARIANCES
Ho:
Ha:
level of significance = = .05
sample sizes both = 15
Assumption: =

Sample means = 11.8 and 12.1


Sample standard deviations = 0.1 and 0.2
Sample sizes = 15 and 15

26

DIFFERENCE IN MEANS - EQUAL VARIANCES


Can you detect this difference?

27

DIFFERENCE IN MEANS - EQUAL


VARIANCES

28

DIFFERENCE IN MEANS - unEQUAL


VARIANCES
Same as the Equal Variance case except
the variances are not assumed equal.
How do you know if it is reasonable to
assume that variances are equal OR
unequal?

29

DIFFERENCE IN VARIANCE
HYPOTHESIS TEST

Same example as Difference in Mean:


Sample standard deviations = 0.1 and 0.2
Sample sizes = 15 and 15
**********************************
Null Hypothesis: ratio of variances = 1.0
Alternative: not equal
Computed F statistic = 0.25
P-Value = 0.0140071
Reject the null hypothesis for alpha = 0.05.

30

DIFFERENCE IN VARIANCE
HYPOTHESIS TEST
Can you detect this difference?

31

DIFFERENCE IN VARIANCE
HYPOTHESIS TEST -POWER CURVE

32

PAIRED DIFFERENCE IN MEANS


HYPOTHESIS TEST
Two different inspectors each measure 10
parts on the same piece of test equipment.
Null hypothesis: DIFFERENCE IN MEANS
= 0.0
Alternative: not equal
Computed t statistic = -1.22702
P-Value = 0.250944
Do not reject the null hypothesis for alpha =
0.05.
33

PAIRED DIFFERENCE IN MEANS


HYPOTHESIS TEST - POWER CURVE
Power Curve
alpha = 0.05, sigma = 3.866

Power

0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
-5

-4

-3

-2

-1

Difference in Means
34

ONE WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE


Used to test hypothesis that the means of
several populations are equal.
Example: Production line has 7 fill needles and
you wish to assess whether or not the average
fill is the same for all 7 needles.
Experiment: sample 20 fills from each of the 9
needles and test at 5% level of sign.
Ho: =
35

RESULTS: ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE


TABLE

Analysis of Variance
---------------------------------------------------------------------------Source
Sum of Squares
Df Mean Square
F-Ratio
P-Valu
---------------------------------------------------------------------------Between groups
1.10019
6
0.183364
18.66
0.000
Within groups
1.30717
133
0.00982837
---------------------------------------------------------------------------Total (Corr.)
2.40736
139

36

SINCE NEEDLE MEANS ARE NOT ALL


EQUAL, WHICH ONES ARE DIFFERENT?
Multiple Range Tests for 7 Needles
Method: 95.0 percent LSD
Col_2
Count
Mean
Homogeneous Groups
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------N7
20
11.786
X
N2
20
11.9811
X
N1
20
11.9827
X
N6
20
11.9873
X
N3
20
11.9951
X
N5
20
11.9953
X
N4
20
12.11
X

37

VISUAL COMPARISON OF 7
NEEDLES
Box-and-Whisker Plot
N1
N2
Col_2

N3
N4
N5
N6
N7
11.5

11.7

11.9

12.1

12.3

Col_1

38

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS


Experiments involving several factors ( k =
# of factors) where it is necessary to study
the joint effect of these factors on a specific
response.
Each of the factors are set at two levels (a
low level and a high level) which may
be qualitative (machine A/machine B, fan
on/fan off) or quantitative (temperature
800/temperature 900, line speed 4000 per
hour/line speed 5000 per hour).
39

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS


Factors are assumed to be fixed (fixed
effects model)
Designs are completely randomized
(experimental trials are run in a random
order, etc.)
The usual normality assumptions are
satisfied.

40

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS


Particularly useful in the early stages of
experimental work when you are likely to
have many factors being investigated and
you want to minimize the number of
treatment combinations (sample size) but, at
the same time, study all k factors in a
complete factorial arrangement (the
experiment collects data at all possible
combinations of factor levels).
41

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS


As k gets large, the sample size will
increase exponentially. If experiment is
replicated, the # runs again increases.
k
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10

# of runs
4
8
16
32
64
128
256
512
1024

42

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2)


Two factors set at two levels (normally
referred to as low and high) would result in
the following design where each level of
factor A is paired with each level of factor
B.
Generalized Settings
RUN Factor A Factor B RESPONSE

Orthogonal Settings
RUN Factor A Factor B RESPONSE

low

low

y1

-1

-1

y1

high

low

y2

+1

-1

y2

low

high

y3

-1

+1

y3

high

high

y4

+1

+1

y4
43

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2)


Estimating main effects associated with
changing the level of each factor from low
to high. This is the estimated effect on the
response variable associated with changing
factor A or B from their low to high values.

( y2 y4 ) ( y1 y3 )
Factor A Effect

2
2

( y3 y4 ) ( y1 y2 )
Factor B Effect

2
2
44

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2):


GRAPHICAL OUTPUT
Neither factor A nor Factor B have an effect
on the response variable.

45

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2):


GRAPHICAL OUTPUT
Factor A has an effect on the response
variable, but Factor B does not.

46

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2):


GRAPHICAL OUTPUT
Factor A and Factor B have an effect on the
response variable.

47

FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2):


GRAPHICAL OUTPUT
Factor B has an effect on the response variable, but only if
factor A is set at the High level. This is called
interaction and it basically means that the effect one factor
has on a response is dependent on the level you set other
factors at. Interactions can be major problems in a DOE if
you fail to account for the interaction when designing your
experiment.

48

EXAMPLE:
FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2)
A microbiologist is interested in the effect
of two different culture mediums [medium 1
(low) and medium 2 (high)] and two
different times [10 hours (low) and 20 hours
(high)] on the growth rate of a particular
CFU [Bugs].

49

EXAMPLE:
FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2)
Since two factors are of interest, k =2, and
we would need the following four runs
resulting in
Generalized Settings
RUN Medium
Time
Growth Rate
1

low

low

17

high

low

15

low

high

38

high

high

39
50

EXAMPLE:
FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2)
Estimates for the medium and time
effects are
Medium effect = [(15+39)/2] [(17 +
38)/2] = -0.5
Time effect = [(38+39)/2] [(17 + 15)/2] =
22.5
51

EXAMPLE:
FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2)

52

EXAMPLE:
FACTORIAL (2k) DESIGNS (k = 2)
A statistical analysis using the appropriate
statistical model would result in the
following information. Factor A (medium)
and Factor B (time)
Type III Sums of Squares
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------Source
Sum of Squares
Df
Mean Square
F-Ratio
P-Value
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------FACTOR A
0.25
1
0.25
0.11
0.7952
FACTOR B
506.25
1
506.25
225.00
0.0424
Residual
2.25
1
2.25
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------Total (corrected)
508.75
3
All F-ratios are based on the residual mean square error.

53

EXAMPLE:
CONCLUSIONS
In statistical language, one would conclude
that factor A (medium) is not statistically
significant at a 5% level of significance
since the p-value is greater than 5% (0.05),
but factor B (time) is statistically significant
at a 5 % level of significance since this pvalue is less than 5%.

54

EXAMPLE:
CONCLUSIONS
In layman terms, this means that we have
no evidence that would allow us to
conclude that the medium used has an effect
on the growth rate, although it may well
have an effect (our conclusion was
incorrect).

55

EXAMPLE:
CONCLUSIONS
Additionally, we have evidence that would
allow us to conclude that time does have an
effect on the growth rate, although it may
well not have an effect (our conclusion was
incorrect).

56

EXAMPLE:
CONCLUSIONS
In general we control the likelihood of
reaching these incorrect conclusions by the
selection of the level of significance for the
test and the amount of data collected
(sample size).

57

2k DESIGNS (k > 2)
As the number of factors increase, the
number of runs needed to complete a
complete factorial experiment will increase
dramatically. The following 2k design
layout depict the number of runs needed for
values of k from 2 to 5. For example, when
k = 5, it will take 25 = 32 experimental runs
for the complete factorial experiment.

58

Interactions for 2k Designs (k = 3)

Interactions between various factors can


be estimated for different designs above
by multiplying the appropriate columns
together and then subtracting the average
response for the lows from the average
response for the highs.

59

Interactions for 2k Designs (k = 3)


a
-1
+1
-1
+1
-1
+1
-1
+1

b
-1
-1
+1
+1
-1
-1
+1
+1

c
-1
-1
-1
-1
+!
+1
+1
+1

ab
1
-1
-1
1
1
-1
-1
1

ac
1
-1
1
-1
-1
1
-1
1

bc
1
1
-1
-1
-1
-1
1
1

abc
-1
1
1
-1
1
-1
-1
1
60

2k DESIGNS (k > 2)
Once the effect for all factors and
interactions are determined, you are able to
develop a prediction model to estimate the
response for specific values of the factors.
In general, we will do this with statistical
software, but for these designs, you can do
it by hand calculations if you wish.
61

2k DESIGNS (k > 2)
For example, if there are no significant interactions
present, you can estimate a response by the
following formula. (for quantitative factors only)

62

ONE FACTOR EXAMPLE


Plot of Fitted Model
95

GRADE

85
75
65
55
10

12

14

16

#HRS STUDY

18

20
63

ONE FACTOR EXAMPLE


The output shows the results of fitting a
general linear model to describe the
relationship between GRADE and #HRS
STUDY. The equation of the fitted general
model is
GRADE = 29.3 + 3.1* (#HRS STUDY)
The fitted orthogonal model is
GRADE = 75 + 15 * (SCALED # HRS)
64

Two Level Screening Designs


Suppose that your brainstorming session
resulted in 7 factors that various people
think might have an effect on a response.
A full factorial design would require 2 7 =
128 experimental runs without replication.
The purpose of screening designs is to
reduce (identify) the number of factors
down to the major role players with a
minimal number of experimental runs. One
way to do this is to use the 23 full factorial
design and use interaction columns for
factors.
65

Note that
* Any factor d effect is now confounded with the a*b

a
-1
+1
-1
+1
-1
+1
-1
+1

interaction
* Any factor e effect is now confounded with the a*c
interaction
* etc.
* What
is the d*e
interaction
confounded
with????????
b
c
d = ab
e = ac
f = bc
g = abc
-1
-1
+1
+1
-1
-1
+1
+1

-1
-1
-1
-1
+!
+1
+1
+1

1
-1
-1
1
1
-1
-1
1

1
-1
1
-1
-1
1
-1
1

1
1
-1
-1
-1
-1
1
1

-1
1
1
-1
1
-1
-1
1
66

Problems that Interactions Cause!

Interactions If interactions exist and you fail to


account for this, you may reach erroneous
conclusions. Suppose that you plan an
experiment with four runs and three factors
resulting in the following data:

67

Problems that Interactions Cause!


Factor A Effect = 0
Factor B Effect = 0
In this example, if you were assuming that
smaller is better then it appears to make
no difference where you set factors A and B.
If you were to set factor A at the low value
and factor B at the low value, your response
variable would be larger than desired. In this
case there is a factor A interaction with
factor B.
68

Problems that Interactions Cause!

Interaction Plot

RESPONSE

10

FACTOR B
-1
1

9
8
7
6
5

-1

FACTOR A

69

Resolution of a Design

Resolution III Designs No main effects are


aliased with any other main effect BUT some (or
all) main effects are aliased with two way
interactions
Resolution IV Designs No main effects are
aliased with any other main effect OR two factor
interaction, BUT two factor interactions may be
aliased with other two factor interactions
Resolution V Designs No main effect OR two
factor interaction is aliased with any other main
effect or two factor interaction, BUT two factor
interactions are aliased with three factor
interactions.
70

Common Screening Designs

Fractional Factorial Designs the total


number of experimental runs must be a
power of 2 (4, 8, 16, 32, 64, ). If you
believe first order interactions are small
compared to main effects, then you could
choose a resolution III design. Just
remember that if you have major
interactions, it can mess up your screening
experiment.
71

Common Screening Designs

Plackett-Burman Designs Two level,


resolution III designs used to study up to
n-1 factors in n experimental runs, where
n is a multiple of 4 ( # of runs will be 4, 8,
12, 16, ). Since n may be quite large,
you can study a large number of factors
with moderately small sample sizes. (n =
100 means you can study 99 factors with
100 runs)
72

Other Design Issues


May want to collect data at center points to
estimate non-linear responses
More than two levels of a factor no
problem (multi-level factorial)
What do you do if you want to build a nonlinear model to optimize the response.
(hit a target, maximize, or minimize)
called response surface modeling
73

Response Surface Designs Box-Behnken


RUN

F1

F2

F3

Y100

10

45

60

11825

30

45

40

8781

20

30

40

8413

10

30

50

9216

20

45

50

9288

30

60

50

8261

20

45

50

9329

30

45

60

10855

20

45

50

9205

10

20

60

40

8538

11

10

45

40

9718

12

30

30

50

11308

13

20

60

60

10316

14

10

60

50

12056

15

20

30

60

10378

74

Response Surface Designs Box-Behnken


Regression coeffs. for Var_3
---------------------------------------------------------------------constant
= 2312.5
A:Factor_A = 36.575
B:Factor_B = 200.067
C:Factor_C = 3.85
AA
= 9.09875
AB
= -9.81167
AC
= -0.0825
BB
= 0.117222
BC
= -0.311667
CC
= 1.10875

75

Response Surface Designs Box-Behnken

Contours of Estimated Response Surface


Factor_C=60.0

60

Factor_B

55
50
45
40
35
30
10

14

18

22

Factor_A

26

30

Var_3
9300.0
9500.0
9700.0
9900.0
10100.0
10300.0
10500.0
10700.0
10900.0
11100.0
11300.0
11500.0
11700.0
76

CLASSROOM EXERCISE

STUDENT IN-CLASS EXPERIMENT:


Collect data for experiment to determine
factor settings (two factors) to hit a target
response (spot on wall).
Factor A height of shaker (low and high)
Factor B location of shaker (close to
hand and close to wall)
Design experiment would suggest
several replications
77

CLASSROOM EXERCISE
Conduct Experiment student holds 3 foot
pin the tail on the donkey stick and
attempts to hit the target. An observer will
assist to mark the hit on the target.
Collect data students take data home for
week and come back with what you would
recommend AND why.
YOU TELL THE CLASS HOW TO PLAY
THE GAME TO WIN.
78

CLASSROOM EXERCISE

79

CLASSROOM EXERCISE
MARKER VERTICAL
1ST OBS 2ND OBS 3RD OBS 4TH OBS
STICK
POLE

MEAN

STANDARD
DEVIATION

-2.750

-4.500

-4.750

-5.000

-4.250

1.021

-12.500

-6.750

-4.625

-4.000

-6.969

3.871

3.000

3.250

3.875

6.250

4.094

1.484

4.625

11.250

12.625

14.000

10.625

4.155

MARKER
STICK

L = VERTICAL POLE WAS CLOSE TO WALL (MARKER END OF STICK


H=VERTICAL POLE WAS CLOSE TO HAND

VERTICAL
POLE

L=SHAKING DEVICE LOCATED LOW ON VERTICAL POLE


H=SHAKING DEVICE LOCATED HIGH ON VERTICAL POLE

80

Contour Plots for Mean and Std. Dev.

81

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen