Sie sind auf Seite 1von 34

Probability, Bayes Theorem and the Monty Hall Problem

Probability Distributions
A random variable is a variable whose value is uncertain. For example, the height of a randomly selected person in this class is a random variable I wont know its value until the person is selected. Note that we are not completely uncertain about most random variables.
For example, we know that height will probably be in the 5-6 range. In addition, 56 is more likely than 50 or 60 (for women).

The function that describes the probability of each possible value of the random variable is called a probability distribution.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Probability Distributions
Probability distributions are closely related to frequency distributions.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Probability Distributions
Dividing each frequency by the total number of scores and multiplying by 100 yields a percentage distribution.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Probability Distributions
Dividing each frequency by the total number of scores yields a probability distribution.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Probability Distributions
For a discrete distribution, the probabilities over all possible values of the random variable must sum to 1.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Probability Distributions
For a discrete distribution, we can talk about the probability of a particular score occurring, e.g., p(Province = Ontario) = 0.36.

We can also talk about the probability of any one of a subset of scores occurring, e.g., p(Province = Ontario or Quebec) = 0.50.
In general, we refer to these occurrences as events.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Probability Distributions
For a continuous distribution, the probabilities over all possible values of the random variable must integrate to 1 (i.e., the area under the curve must be 1). Note that the height of a continuous distribution can exceed 1!
Shaded area = 0.683 Shaded area = 0.954 Shaded area = 0.997

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Continuous Distributions
For continuous distributions, it does not make sense to talk about the probability of an exact score.
e.g., what is the probability that your height is exactly 65.485948467 inches?
Normal Approximation to probability distribution for height of Canadian females (parameters from General Social Survey, 1991)

0.16 0.14 Probability p 0.12 0.1

5'3.8" s 2.6"

0.08
0.06 0.04 0.02 0 55 60 65 Height (in)
9

70

75

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Continuous Distributions
It does make sense to talk about the probability of observing a score that falls within a certain range
e.g., what is the probability that you are between 53 and 57? e.g., what is the probability that you are less than 510?

Valid events

Normal Approximation to probability distribution for height of Canadian females (parameters from General Social Survey, 1991)

0.16 0.14 Probability p 0.12 0.1

5'3.8" s 2.6"

0.08
0.06 0.04 0.02 0 55 60 65 Height (in)
10

70

75

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Probability of Combined Events


Let p( A) represent the probability of event A. 0 p( A) 1
If A and B are disjoint (mutually exclusive) events, then p( A or B ) p( A) p(B )

Example: in the context of the Community Health Survey: Let A represent the event that the respondent lives in Alberta. Let B represent the event that the respondent lives in BC.

Then p( A) 0.087 p(B ) 0.106 p( A or B ) 0.193


PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER
11

Probability of Combined Events


More generally, if A and B are not mutually exclusive, p( A or B ) p( A) p(B ) p( A and B )

Example: Canadian Community Health Survey, Sleeping Habits

Let A event that respondent sleeps less than 6 hours per night.
Let B event that respondent reports trouble sleeping most or all of the time

p( A) 0.139 p(B) 0.152 p( A and B) 0.061


Thus p( A or B ) 0.139 0.152 0.061 0.230
PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER
12

Exhaustive Events
Two or more events are said to be exhaustive if at least one of them must occur. For example, if A is the event that the respondent sleeps less than 6 hours per night and B is the event that the respondent sleeps at least 6 hours per night, then A and B are exhaustive. (Although A is probably the more exhausted!!)

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

13

Independence
Two events are independent if the occurence of one in no way affects the probability of the other. If events A and B are independent, then p( A and B ) p( A)p(B ) If events A and B are not independent, then p( A and B ) p( A) p(B | A)

Example: pick a card, any card.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

14

An Example: The Monty Hall Problem

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

15

Problem History
When problem first appeared in Parade, approximately 10,000 readers, including 1,000 PhDs, wrote claiming the solution was wrong. In a study of 228 subjects, only 13% chose to switch.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

16

Intuition
Before Monty opens any doors, there is a 1/3 probability that the car lies behind the door you selected (Door 1), and a 2/3 probability it lies behind one of the other two doors. Thus with 2/3 probability, Monty will be forced to open a specific door (e.g., the car lies behind Door 2, so Monty must open Door 3). This concentrates all of the 2/3 probability in the remaining door (e.g., Door 2).

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

17

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

18

Analysis
Car hidden behind Door 1 Car hidden behind Door 2 Car hidden behind Door 3

Player initially picks Door 1

Host opens either Door 2 or 3

Host must open Door 3

Host must open Door 2

Switching loses with probability 1/6

Switching loses with probability 1/6

Switching wins with probability 1/3

Switching wins with probability 1/3

Switching loses with probability 1/3


PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

Switching wins with probability 2/3

19

Notes
It is important that
Monty must open a door that reveals a goat
Monty cannot open the door you selected

These rules mean that your choice may constrain what Monty does.
If you initially selected a door concealing a goat, then there is only one door Monty can open.

One can rigorously account for the Monty Hall problem using a Bayesian analysis

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

20

End of Lecture 2
Sept 17, 2008

Conditional Probability
To understand Bayesian inference, we first need to understand the concept of conditional probability. What is the probability I will roll a 12 with a pair of (fair) dice? What if I first roll one die and get a 6? What now is the probability that when I roll the second die they will sum to 12?
Let A be the state of die 1 Let B be the state of die 2 Let C be the sum of die 1 and 2

p( A 6) __? p(B 6) __? p(C 12) __? p(C 12 | A 6) __?


Probability of C given A

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

22

Conditional Probability
The conditional probability of A given B is the joint probability of A and B, divided by the marginal probability of B.
p( A, B ) p( A | B ) p(B )

Thus if A and B are statistically independent,


p( A | B ) p( A, B ) p( A)p(B ) p( A). p(B ) p(B )

However, if A and B are statistically dependent, then


p( A | B ) p( A).
PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER
23

Bayes Theorem
Bayes Theorem is simply a consequence of the definition of conditional probabilities:
p( A, B ) p( A | B ) p( A, B ) p( A | B )p(B ) p(B ) p( A, B ) p(B | A) p( A, B ) p(B | A)p( A) p( A)
Thus p( A | B)p(B) p(B | A)p( A)
p(B | A)p( A) p( A | B ) p(B )
PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER
24

Bayes Equation

Bayes Theorem
Bayes theorem is most commonly used to estimate the state of a hidden, causal variable H based on the measured state of an observable variable D:
Likelihood Prior

p(H | D )

p(D | H )p(H ) p(D )

Posterior

Evidence

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

25

Bayesian Inference
Whereas the posterior p(H|D) is often difficult to estimate directly, reasonable models of the likelihood p(D|H) can often be formed. This is typically because H is causal on D. Thus Bayes theorem provides a means for estimating the posterior probability of the causal variable H based on observations D.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

26

Marginalizing
To calculate the evidence p(D) in Bayes equation, we typically have to marginalize over all possible states of the causal variable H.
p(D | H )p(H ) p(H | D ) p(D )

p(D) p(D, H1 ) p(D, H2 )

p(D, Hn ) p(D | Hn )p(Hn )

p(D | H1 )p(H1 ) p(D | H2 )p(H2 )

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

27

The Full Monty


Lets get back to The Monty Hall Problem.

Lets assume you initially select Door 1.


Suppose that Monty then opens Door 2 to reveal a goat. We want to calculate the posterior probability that a car lies behind Door 1 after Monty has provided these new data.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

28

The Full Monty


Let Ci represent the state that the car lies behind Door i , i [1 ,2,3].

Let Mi represent the event that Monty opens door i , i [1 ,2,3], revealing a goat.
We seek p(C1 | M2 ) p(M2 | C1 )p(C1 ) p(M2 )

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

29

The Full Monty


Since p(C2 | M2 ) 0, we can obtain p(C3 | M2 ) by subtracting p(C1 | M2 ) from 1 (Remember that the probabilities of exhaustive events add to 1!)
However, we can also calculate p(C3 | M2 ) directly:

p(C3 | M 2 )

p(M 2 | C3 )p(C3 ) p(M2 )

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

30

But were not on Lets Make a Deal!


Why is the Monty Hall Problem Interesting?
It reveals limitations in human cognitive processing of uncertainty
It provides a good illustration of many concepts of probability It get us to think more carefully about how we deal with and express uncertainty as scientists.

What else is Bayes theorem good for?

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

31

Clinical Example
Christiansen et al (2000) studied the mammogram results of 2,227 women at health centers of Harvard Pilgrim Health Care, a large HMO in the Boston metropolitan area.
The women received a total of 9,747 mammograms over 10 years. Their ages ranged from 40 to 80. Ninety-three different radiologists read the mammograms, and overall they diagnosed 634 mammograms as suspicious that turned out to be false positives. This is a false positive rate of 6.5%. The false negative rate has been estimated at 10%.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

32

Clinical Example
There are about 58,500,000 women between the ages of 40 and 80 in the US The incidence of breast cancer in the US is about 184,200 per year, i.e., roughly 1 in 318.

PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER

33

Clinical Example
Let C0 represent the absence of cancer. Let C1 represent the presence of cancer.
Let M0 represent a negative mammogram result. Let M1 represent a positive mammogram result.
Suppose your friend receives a positive mammogram result. What quantity do you want to compute?

Remember: p(C1 | M1 ) p(M1 | C1 )!


PSYC 6130, PROF. J. ELDER
34

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen