Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

Sound Localization in the Median Plane *

by J. BLAUERT

Institut fur elektrische Nachrichtentechnik der Rheinisch-Westfalischen


Technischen Hochschule Aachen
Summary
Psychoacoustic measurements with observers who were stimulated at both ears with iden-
tical narrow band signals yielded the following results:
The sound sensations of the observers were localized in the median plane. The direction
of the sound sensation is a function of frequency only and does not depend on the angle
of incidence.
Physical measurements of the linear distortions caused by the pinna showed further,
that the pinna and the hearing system behind the pinna work together in such a way, that
sound localization of broad band signals in the median plane can also be explained.
Zum Richtungshoren in der Medianebene
Zusammenfassung
Psychoakustische Messungen an Versuchspersonen, deren beide Ohren mit identi-
schem Schmalbandrauschen beschallt wurden, fiihrten zu folgendem Ergebnis:
Die Horereignisse der Versuchspersonen sind in der Medianebene lokalisiert. Die Rich-
tung der Horereignisse ist eine Funktion der Frequenz, sie ist von der Schalleinfallsrich-
tung unabhangig.
Physikalische Messungen der linearen Verzerrungen an den Ohrmuscheln zeigten wei-
terhin, daB die Ohrmuscheln und die nachgeschalteten Gehbrteile in soldier Weise zusam-
menarbeiten, daB das Richtungshoren in der Medianebene auch fur Breitbandsignale er-
klart werden kann.
Localisation Sonore dans le Plan Median
Sommaire
Des mesures psychoacoustiques sur des sujets dont les deux oreilles etaient exposees a
des signaux a bande etroite identique ont donne les resultats suivants:
Les sensations sonores des sujets sont localisees dans le plan median. La direction de la
sensation auditive est uniquement fonction de la frequence et independante de Tangle
d'incidence.
Les mesures physiques des distortions lineaires au niveau du pavilion de l'oreille sont
demontre que le pavilion et les elements du systeme auditif qui le suivent collaborent de
maniere a permettre egalement la localisation sonore des signaux de large bande de fre-
quence dans le plan median.

1. Introduction Even the function between the angle of incidence


of the sound waves and the interaural signal dif-
Sound sensations, like the phenomena of all other ferences cannot be described unambiguously. This
senses, are more or less precisely located in space. fact is illustrated in Fig. 1 by the example of inter-
The psychoacoustic intention concerning this fact is aural time differences.
to find out quantitatively the relations between the
location of the sound sensations and those physical
parameters that contribute to the formation of this
location.
The following article deals with the direction of
the sound sensations. The direction is normally sup-
posed to be determined by interaural differences of
the sound signals at the eardrums. These interaural right ear
differences depend on the angle of incidence of the
sound waves which strike the observers heads.
However, a well-defined function between the
interaural signal differences and the direction of the
sound sensations cannot be given. Fig. 1. Geometric locations of sound sources which pro-
duce identical interaural time-differences
* From a dissertation, accepted at the Rheinisch- (a) in the plane,
Westfalische Technische Hochschule Aachen, 1969. (b) in space.
ACUSTICA
206 J. BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION Vol. 22 (1969/70)

Using a simple model of VON HORNBOSTEL and observer undergoes linear distortions by diffrac-
WERTHEIMER [8], the ears of the observers are tion-, resonance- and shadow effects caused by head,
represented by two points being at a distance of pinna and ear-canal (WIENER and Ross [18] and
approximately 21 cm from each other. It can be others). Thus, the spectrum of the signal at the ear-
shown that all sound sources situated on the surface drum varies with the angle of incidence what can
of a rotational hyperboloid as shown in the figure be noticed by the observer by a variation of the
produce the same interaural time differences at the timbre of his sound sensation.
ears. For a zero time difference, the hyperboloid It is obvious to assume, that the same spectral
turns into the median plane. cues that are responsible for the timbre also in-
Since, in spite of the ambiguous relations of inter- fluence the location of the sound sensation. Many
aural signal differences and the angle of sound experimental results are reported which are con-
incidence, the direction of the sound sensations is sistent with this theory (HECHT [7], KiETZ [9],
unambiguously determined, further information BATTEAU [1], ROFFLER and BUTLER [13] and
must be perceived by the observers. many others) but there remains one question which
It is assumed by many authors (originally by makes some authors doubt the whole theory [5],
VAN SOEST [14], later for example by KLENSCH [9], [13]:
[10], DE BOER [4], WALLACH [17] and THURLOW If the direction of the sound sensation is indeed
and RtTNGE [16], that small head movements and determined by spectral cues which are recognized by
accompanying changes in the interaural signal timbre, one should assume, that a "reference-timbre"
differences during sound perception are evaluated must be known to the observer. Correct localization
to form the direction of the sound sensation. (which means spatial coincidence of sound source
This assumption, though surely correct and suf- and sound sensation) would presume a sound signal
ficiently proven, does not give a full explanation of that is familiar to the observer. Experiments rather
the localization of sound sensations. Two arguments show that this is not necessary. The only necessary
can be named to explain this deficiency: condition is, that the sound signal must be of suf-
1) Unambiguously located sound sensations occur ficient bandwidth.
even when the sound signal is so short that no The theory of bone conduction pays regard to the
head movements take place (VON WlLMOWSKY two paths by which sound can reach the inner ear
[19] reports a lag of 360 ms). namely by the ear canal, ear-drum etc. and by bone
2) Unambiguously located sound sensations are ob- conduction via the skull. It is assumed by some
served even when the head is mechanically fixed authors (HECHT [7], SONE [15]), that the portion
(KIETZ [9], BURGER [5] and others). of sound that reaches the inner ear by bone conduc-
For further examinations it is most appropriate tion varies with the angle of incident and so con-
to use test arrangements, in which the sound sources tributes to the formation of the direction of the
are situated in or symmetrically to the median plane. sound sensation.
In such arrangements no perceivable interaural
However, experiments with low level signals
signal differences appear, if head movements larger
(below the perception level for bone conduction)
than about 1° are suppressed [3].
and others with masked bone conduction (skull
If observers are stimulated in this way, their
stimulated by noise) did not support the hypothesis
sound sensations are located in the median plane.
that bone conduction is a necessary cue for localiza-
The cues which determine the direction of the sound
tion in the median plane.
sensations must then be the same at both ears. The
problem to detect those cues is reduced to a mon- The visual, tactile and vestibular theories are
aural one. heterosensoric theories, since they assume that hear-
ing alone cannot detect the location of the sound
source in order to determine the correct direction of
2. The theories of localization in the median the sound sensation.
plane
Visual stimuli which are most important for
To explain the localization in the median plane, localization in general also strongly influence
several theories have been discussed in the literature. auditory localization. Experiments, proving this,
The more important ones are have been reported for example by GtJTTlCH [6],
(1) the theory of timbre differences, WALLACH [17], ROFFLER and BUTLER [12] and by
(2) the theory of bone conduction, [3].
(3) the theories of visual, tactile and vestibular cues. Tactile stimuli which are supposed to be perceived
The theory of timbre differences starts from the by the pinna or by the skin of the neck do not seem
fact that a sound wave which strikes the head of an to be important for auditory localization. GtJTTlCH
ACUSTICA
Vol. 22 (1969/70) J. BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION 207

reports, that narcotization of the pinna did not in- The duration of the test signals was changed be-
jure auditory localization. Experiments of the author tween 100ms and I s ; the sound pressure levels
in which the neck of the observer was covered with were 30 dB, 40 dB, 50 dB and 60 dB above 2.10" 4
felt, did not show significant results either. [xbar. The signal-to-noise-ratio was better than
Vestibular cues play a role in auditory localiza- 65 dB; the distortion factor was less than 2%.
tion at least when head movements are admitted Switching from one loudspeaker arrangement to
(WALLACH [17], NEURMAN and MEURMAN [11] another did not produce any click.
and others). On the other hand when the head is The test signals were presented in stochastic order
fixed influence of vestibular cues could hardly be relative to the centre frequencies of the third octave
discriminated with non-physiological methods. Yet bands, the sound pressure levels and the loudspeaker
one cannot exclude the possibility that parts of the arrangements. In each of the test series 5 to 20 male
vestibular organ (for example the sacculus) are observers (19 to 30 year old) with normal hearing
stimulable by sound and so support the auditory took part who had been audiometrically tested. All
localization. observers were able to localize "correctly" broad
Anyway it must be assumed that sound localiza- band noise signals coming from the front, the rear,
tion in the median plane and lateralization are based the left or the right. In one test series a small brass
on different physiological processes [2]. The results tube (5 cm long and 0.6 cm in diameter) was plug-
of three experiments of the author, reported in the ged into each of the observers two ear-canals.
next paragraphs, show that a modified theory of Preliminary experiments had shown that with the
timbre differences can give a new view on localiza- speaker arrangements of Fig. 1 all sound sensations
tion of sound sensations in the median plane. appeared in the median plane and never more than
15° below the horizontal plane. The upper part of
the median plane was divided into three sectors "v",
3. First experiment*
" o " and "h" (Fig. 3). The observers had to estimate
3.1. Procedure ..0"-

In a darkened, anechoic chamber observers were


stimulated by noise signals of one third octave band-
width. The presentation of the stimuli was done
alternatively by five different arrangements of sound
sources (Fig. 2). The heads of the observers were
slightly clamped. Head movements did not exceed
0.8°. The stimuli arriving at the two ears of the ob-
servers could therefore be regarded as nearly
identical.

Fig. 3. Nominal scale of the observers to describe the


direction of the sound sensations.
in which of these three sectors their sound sensation
appeared. This judgment had to be given within
10 s.
3.2. Results
Fig. 4 shows the record of measurements with one
observer who was stimulated by the front and rear
loudspeaker. It is obvious that in some frequency
bands the judgments "h", "v" resp. " o " accumulate
1a
independently of the direction of incidence.
In the Figs. 5, 6 and 7 the results of simular
Fig. 2. Different loudspeaker arrangements which pro- measurements with 20 observers are summarized.
duce identical signals at both ears, All three diagrams show a clear dependency of the
la = front, lb = rear, lc = above, relative frequency of each of the judgments on the
2,2' = left and right together, 3 = head- centre frequency of the third octave bands.
phones.
Further statistical treatments show, that a signifi-
1
Results of the first experiment have partly been cant dependency of the relative frequencies of judg-
reported by the author on the 6th ICA, Tokyo 1968. ments neither on the direction of sound incidence
ACUSTICA
208 J. BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION Vol. 22 (1969/70)

Fig. 4.
H<TJ von vorne K->trLx . von hinten Record of measurements with one observer.
CO cingosp'ielt
eing?spieit
09 (A) Loudspeaker in front,
prel in dB VfK«1 in dB (B) loudspeaker in the rear.
JO Mitten-
40 50 «0 30 40 50 60
M
V V V V V . V V V
125 Ha
V V V V V ' V V V
KM
160 Ifa
V V V V V V V V
200 lU
u I*
V V V V V V V V
290 Hi
n — • *

V i V v V
315 H* V • v V y
* >» *tt sr
V V V V V V V V
400 H2
It a */» t*r Si
V V V V 500 Hi V V V V

V h h V 6J0 Hz ' h h V h
h h h " h 800 Hz
h h "h h
nt •4?

h h h h 1000 He h h h h
w
h h h h h h h
1250 Hs
" h h h V
1600 Hi
h * h * h * h
a,
h h *. h * V
2000 Hz h h h
««• -JH irt
" h V V V V V V V
2500 Hi
i* So tn
V V V V V V V V
5150 Ha
•ft It Jtf '
u\ H
V V V V V V V
*000 H* fp*
V
-n 'id 11
V V V V V V V
V 5000 Hz
41 J *»»
v , V -v f>500 Hz
' v V r 0
411 V

<«* r o •»
' h 0000 Hz h 0 ^ 0
ta W
'h loOOO Hz
" h h
v
" h 12500 Hs
"' h h
w w
V V " V •* v v v
16000 Hs

100

L75

\W

0
125 250 500 Hz 1 2 8 kHz 16 500 Hz 1 2 8 kHz 16
frequency frequency •

Fig. 5. Relative frequency of "v"-judgements (20 ob- Fig. 6. Relative frequency of "o"-judgements (20 ob-
servers, each stimulated once from the front, servers, each stimulated once from the front,
once from the rear, in each of the 1/3 octave once from the rear, in each of the 1/3 octave
bands). bands).
— 60 dB, 60 dB,
50 dB, 50 dB,
_._._ 40 dB, — — 40 dB,
30 dB. 30 dB.

nor on the sound pressure level or the signal dura- tubes in the ear-canals led to results which are fairly
tion can be proven. identical to those of the front-back-test without brass
The hearing tests with the other loudspeaker tubes as shown in Figs. 5, 6, 7, so that they need not
arrangements of Fig. 1 including those with brass be presented here in detail.
ACUSTICA
Vol. 22 (1969/70)
J. BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION 209

a set of judgments obtained from measurements with


one representative observer are plotted against stan-
dard deviations of a groups' judgments in Fig. 9.
I greater at a
T 95% level of significance

500 Hz 1
frequency
8 kHz 16
-

i1 v o h
~2000Hz
v o h
4000 Hz
v o h

Fig. 9. Standard deviation of the judgements "v", "o",


8000 Hz

Fig. 7. Relative frequency of "h"-judgements (20 ob-


servers, each stimulated once from the front, "h"
once from the rear, in each of the 1/3 octave "white areas": of one trained observer in 10
bands). tests,
60 dB, "shaded areas": of 10 observers in 1 test each.
50 dB, (8 stimuli per 1/3 octave band in each test.)
_ . _ . _ 40 dB,
30 dB. The diagram points to the fact that the limits of the
directional bands of the individual observers under-
In Fig. 8 the results of a nonparametric statistical lie inter-individual variations; however, in the cen-
evaluation of the data are plotted. It can be shown tre parts of the directional bands inter-individual
that the absolute majority of a population of ob- agreement is quite good.
servers will in certain frequency bands significantly
prefer one of the judgments to the sum of both of
the others. The bands in which such preferences 4. Second experiment
occur are shown on top of Fig. 8 as "directional" 4.1. Procedure
bands.
WIENER and Ross [18] were the first who mea-
directional bands sured the linear distortions caused by head, pinna
and ear-canal. They used a probe microphone to
measure the sound pressure near the ear-drum and
at the entrance of the ear-canal in relation to the
sound pressure in the free field.
In this connection they pointed out that the trans-
mission factor from the entrance of the ear-canal to
the ear-drum does not depend on the angle of sound
incidence. In order to measure the influence of the
angle of sound incidence, it is therefore sufficient to
measure the sound pressure at the ear-canal entrance.
We did such measurements during stimulation by
0 the above, the front and the rear loudspeaker of
125 250 500 Hz 1 2 8 kHz 16
frequency — Fig. 1. The experimental make up is shown in
Fig. 10.
Fig. 8. Relative frequencies of observers who give one
of the judgements "v", "o" or "h" more fre-
quently than both of the others together (at a
4.2. Results
95% level of significance).
The "directional" bands, presented on top of Fig. 11 shows the mean sound pressure level at
the diagram, are those bands, in which the ab- the ear-canal entrances of ten observers when stimu-
solute majority of the population of observers lated by the front speaker minus the mean sound
gives one of the judgements more frequently pressure level at the same point when likewise
than both of the others together:
"bordered areas": at a 90% level of significance, stimulated by the rear speaker. The plotted curve is
"shaded areas": most likely. identical with the corresponding curve to be mea-
sured at the ear-drums.
To prove the "objectivity" of the evaluated posi- It can be seen from the diagram that in some
tions of the directional bands standard deviations of frequency bands the level at the ear-drum is higher
ACUSTICA
210 J.BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION Vol. 22 (1969/70)

boosted bands
JM-
Fr re fr

III V

NV
125 250 500 Hz 1 8 kHz 16
frequency
Fig. 11. SPL at the eardrum with loudspeaker in front
minus SPL at the eardrum with loudspeaker in
the rear.
(Mean of 10 observers and confidence inter-
val of the mean at a 95% level of significance.)
The "boosted" bands, presented on top of the
diagram, are those bands, in which the majo-
rity of the population of observers has a higher
SPL at the eardrum when stimulated from the
front (fr) resp. from the rear (re) than when
stimulated from the opposite direction:
"bordered areas": at a 95% level of signifi-
cance,
"shaded areas": most likely.

avoids any stimulation errors on principle (Fig. 12).


The steps are as follows:
(a) An electroacoustic canal including a probe
microphone was built. By the help of an equal-
Fig. 10. Sound pressure measurements with a probe izer the over-all-transmission-factor could be
microphone at the entrance of the ear-canal. adjusted to be nearly flat ( ± 3 dB) within a
(The sound field near the pinna is hardly
distorted.) frequency range of 0.1 to 16 kHz. A pink noise
generator was connected to the canal input.
during frontal stimulation than in other bands dur- (b) An observer was placed into the air section of
ing stimulation from the rear. After generalization the canal so that the loudspeaker was in front
of these results for a population of observers, the of him and the tip of the tube of the probe
boosted bands shown at the top of Fig. 11 can be microphone lay at the entrance of one of his
specified. Stimulation from above produces another ear-canals. The output signal was now no longer
boosted band at 8 kHz. a pink noise like in case (a) but a pink noise
linearly distorted by the observers' pinna. This
5. Third experiment output signal was recorded on tape.
(c) The same as under (b) but with the observer
5.1. Procedure having his back to the loudspeaker. The output
The aim of this experiment was to prove the hypo- signal was again recorded.
thesis of the theory of timbre differences: that the (d) Another electroacoustic canal was provided for
linear distortions which occur in the sound signal the reproduction of the recorded signals. The
when it strikes the head and passes the pinna on its transmission factor was now adjusted in such
way to the ear-drum, really give the information a way that it was flat between the input of the
which is needed to form the "correct" direction of canal and the ear-canal entrance of the same
sound sensation. Experiments of a similar kind observer who took part in the previous steps of
have been reported by KIETZ [9], BATTEAU [1] the experiment. For technical reasons (signal-
and others who let the sound signals pass through to-noise-ratio) two loudspeakers were used in
pinna imitations. this part of the experiment. The adjustment of
The procedure reported in this article uses the the equalizer was controlled by means of the
natural pinnas of the observers as filters and so probe microphone.
ACUSTICA
Vol. 22 (1969/70)
J. BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION 211

Fig. 12.
Explanation of symbols: Generation of special linear distort-
(a) transmission canal with a transmission factor f independent of frequency for 1/3 octave level diagrams: ed signals by using natural ears as
J L ...undistorted signal filters and stimulation of observers
noise- equalizer loud- probe- output n signal distorted with these signals.
generator speaker microphone signal J L ...by pinna
p signal distorted
J I by pinna
for logarithmic
pinna inserted into the canal, sound from front transmission factors :
^ ' " ' L ...linear distortion
h equalization .,
J ^^... of ^ - ' ' ' L
linear distortion
. by pinna
© pinna inserted into the canal f sound from the rear r\.. linear distortion
I . by pinna
N—ff]—m C=H^-—-fool linear distortion
. by pinna

n equalization
of

for reproduction (transmission factor independent of frequency)

(c) stimulation of observer with output signal

stimulation of observer with output signal ( c )

(e), (f) The lineary distorted noise signals recorded was reproduced that was originally recorded during
under (b) and (c) were reproduced over the sound exposure from the rear. Moreover, all of the
canal provided under (d). The observer was observers said that their sound sensations were
asked to describe the direction of his sound precisely located.
sensation immediately after the presentation of Statistically, the agreement of ten observers means
each of the signals. that at a 95% level of significance more than 74% of
The third experiment was performed with 10 ob- a population of observers will behave like the test
servers like those used in the first experiment. They group. The behaviour of the test group can thus be
were without any knowledge of the aim of the test referred to as typical.
and they had not consciously listened to pink noise It should be noticed that the experiment worked
before. During stimulation the anechoic chamber as well when speech or music was used instead of the
was darkened. pink noise.

5.2. Results 6. Discussion and conclusions


All ten test persons showed spontaneously exactly The results of the third experiment support the
the same behaviour. They reported their sound theory of timbre differences. The direction of sound
sensation to be in front, when that signal was re- sensation can be altered merely by altering the spec-
produced that was originally recorded during sound trum of the sound signal that reaches the ear-drum,
exposure from the front, and then reported their at least when visual cues are not available. If the
sound sensation to be in the rear, when that signal spectrum at the ear drum is constant, the direction of
ACUSTICA
212 J. BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION Vol. 22 (1969/70)

sound incidence does not play any role for the direc- The directional bands have been found in tests
tion of the sound sensation. To determine a certain with narrow band signals. It may be assumed now
direction of the sound sensation, the sound signal that they also effect the localization of broad band
must obviously be linearly distorted in such a way signals in such a way that the sound sensation is
as is normally done by head and pinna, when the mainly determined by that class of directional bands,
sound wave reaches the observer from that very in which the most power relative to the other classes
direction from which the sound sensation is desired of directional bands comes in. The cooperation of
to appear. This was proven for the front and rear head and pinna with the other parts of the hearing
direction. system can then be understood as is schematically
The experiment works as well when the sound explained in Fig. 14 on front-back-discrimination:
signals are unknown to the subjects under test. The
linear distorted signal
results of the first and second experiment fit to
explain this.
In the first experiment the observers have been
stimulated by narrow band signals. The effect of
head and pinna on these signals is not an alteration
of different parts of the spectra relative to each other
but only a variation of the sound pressure level. fp fp fr -r, h v h v h v
The results of the experiment show that in narrow
band signals no information about the incidence
direction of the sound is included which could be
fr fp fr h v h v h v
detected by the hearing system. The hearing system
frequency *•
forms the sound sensation in a direction that depends
only on the frequency of the signal. The psycho- Fig. 14. The cooperation of the outer ear with those
physiological function between the signal frequency parts of the hearing system which cause the
directional bands schematically represented
and the direction of the sound sensation can be with three special noise signals.
described by directional bands (Fig. 8).
In the second experiment the filter effect of head In the first column three signals are represented
and pinna has been measured in detail. In the case by their power density spectra. They may hit the
of stimulation by the front resp. rear loudspeaker observer from the front. Head and pinna then form
Fig. 11 shows boosted bands, in which spectral parts a filter that in some way looks like a comb filter
of a signal coming from one direction are boosted (2nd column). The signals passing this filter are
in relation to the corresponding spectral parts of a relatively boosted within the "fr"-bands. The result-
signal coming from the other direction. ing power density spectra of the signals at the ear-
In Fig. 13 the positions of the directional and the drum are shown in the third column.
boosted bands on the frequency axis are confronted. In regard to the assumption that the direction of
It is obvious that the positions of directional "h" the sound sensation is determined by those direc-
tional bands that get most of the signal power, the
directional bands v h v h
sound sensations of the signals of the first and
second row will be in front of the observers. This
II V/A VM will be the same for nearly all usual signals, as long
boosted bands fr as their power density curve is to some extend
125 250 500 Hz 1 2 8 kHz 16
smooth. The subjects under test need no previous
frequency knowledge of the signal.
Only for very unusual signals like those in the
Fig. 13. Positions of the directional bands and the
boosted bands on the frequency axis. third row of Fig. 14 or special low pass or high pass
signals the detection system fails and the direction
bands and boosted "re" bands correspond to each of the sound sensation will be inverted. That these
other. The same can be said about the "v" and "fr" inversions indeed occur exactly as predicted could
bands with the exception of a missing "v"-band at be proven by very extensive hearing tests with
16 kHz. A look at Fig. 8 shows however, that this various especially filtered signals [3].
band may exist and become significant in tests with Similar tests showed what happens when no clear
larger groups of observers. (The "o"-band and its preponderation of power in one the classes of direc-
corresponding boosted band may be neglected here tional bands takes effect. For example, when about
to simplify the discussion.) the same power falls into "v" and "h" bands, the
ACUSTICA
Vol. 22 (1969/70) J. BLAUERT: SOUND LOCALIZATION 213

sound sensation is located in the head or is some- [4] DE BOER, K., A remarkable phenomenon with
times divided into a front and a rear part. If a stereophonic sound reproduction. Philips Tech.
sufficient part of the power falls into the "o" band, Rev. 9 [1947], 8.
[5] BURGER, J. F., Front-back discrimination of the
the sound sensation appears under a certain angle hearing system. Acustica 8 [1958], 301.
of elevation. [6] GiiTTiCH, A., Schallrichtungsbestimmung und
All experimental results reported in this article Vestibularapparat. Arch. Ohren-Nasen- und Kehl-
are consistent with the following hypothesis: kopfheilkunde 142 [1937], 139.
[7] HECHT, H., Uber die Lokalisation von Schall-
Localization in the median plane takes place in quellen. Naturwiss. 10 [1922], 107.
cooperation with the outer ear (head, pinna etc.) [8] v. HORNBOSTEL, E. M. and WERTHEIMER, M.,
and the other parts of the hearing system in the Sitzungsber. Akad. Wissensch. Berlin 20 [1920],
following way: 388.
[9] KIETZ, H., Das raumliche Horen. Acustica 3
The arriving sound signal is filtered by the outer [1953], 73.
ear. This effects a relative boosting of those parts of [10] KLENSCH, H., Die Lokalisation des Schalles im
the signal that lie in certain frequency bands which Raum. Naturwiss. 36 [1949], 145.
depend on the direction of sound incidence. [11] MEURMANN, Y. and MEURMANN, O. H., Do the
If after the filtering the most powerful components semicircular canals play a part in directional
hearing? Acta-Oto-Laryngol. 44 [1954], 542.
of the signal are in those frequency bands that have [12] ROFFLER, K. and BUTLER, R. A., Localization of
been boosted, the sound sensation will be formed in tonal stimuli in the vertical plane. J. Acoust.
a direction that coincides with the direction of sound Soc. Amer. 43 [1968], 1260.
incidence. [13] ROFFLER, S. and BUTLER, R. A., Factors, that
influence the localization of sound in the verti-
cal plane. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 43 [1968],
Acknowledgement 1255.
[14] v. SOEST, J. L., Richtingshooren bij sinusvormige
The author wishes to thank Prof. Dr.-Ing. V. geluidstrillingen. Physica 9 [1929], 271.
ASCHOFF, Aachen and Prof. Dr.-Ing. W. JANOWSKY, [15] SONE, T., On the difference between localization
and lateralisation. Reports of the 6. ICA, Tokyo
Aachen for their promoting interest in this research. 1968, No. A-3-6.
(Received May 23 r d , 1969.) [16] THURLOW, W. R. and RUNGE, P. S., Effects of
induced head movements on localization of direct
sound. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 42 [1967], 480
References and 1347.
[17] WALLACH, H., On sound localization. J. Acoust.
[1] BATTEAU, D. W., The role of the pinna in human Soc. Amer. 10 [1949], 270.
localization. Proc. Roy. Soc. London (B) 168 [18] WIENER, F. M. and Ross, D. A., The pressure
[1967], 158. distribution in the auditory canal in a progressive
[2] BLAUERT, J., Ein Beitrag zur Tragheit des Rich- sound field. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 18 [1946],
tungshbrens in der Horizontalebene. Acustica 20 401.
[1968], 200. [19] v. WILMOWSKY, H. J., Dynamische Vorgange
[3] BLAUERT, J., Untersuchungen zum Richtungs- beim Richtungshoren. Z. Naturforsch. 15 b [1960],
horen in der Medianebene bei fixiertem Kopf. 132.
Dissertation Aachen, 1969. For further references see [3].

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen